Printer Friendly

The Vocalizations and Hearing Sensitivity of an Explosive-Breeding Tropical Toad from Southern China: A Test of the Matched Filter Hypothesis.

Byline: Tongliang Wang, Lele Jia, Xiaofei Zhai, Jianguo Cui and Jichao Wang

Key words

Acoustic properties, Duttaphrynus melanostictus, Hearing sensitivity, Matched filter hypothesis, Sexual selection.

INTRODUCTION

Acoustic communication plays an important role in survival and reproduction (Davies and Halliday, 1978; Bee et al., 1999; Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Cui et al., 2012). Acoustic properties and hearing sensitivity jointly affect acoustic communication (Ron, 2008; Brittan-Powell et al., 2010; Cui et al., 2012). In anurans, acoustic properties (i.e., dominant frequency, note duration) are usually influenced by body size (Cocroft and Ryan, 1995; Tarano, 2001; Wang et al., 2012), suggesting they could facilitate decision-making related to male-male competition and/or female choice. However, some studies have found no relationship between acoustic properties and morphological characteristics, such as snout-vent length and head length (Penna, 2004; Marquez et al., 2005; Cui et al., 2012).

Females receive and analyze the courtship calls produced by males to select suitable mates during the breeding season. Therefore, females may have the sensitive auditory perception to receive and analyze male attractive signals that are under sexual selection (Gall et al., 2011; Forstmeier et al., 2014). Interestingly, studies of the American bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana; Hetherington, 1994), the Hylidae (Eleutherodactylus coqui; Narins and Capranica, 1976), and the Ranidae (Odorrana tormota and O. graminea, Shen et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2016) suggest that hearing sensitivity differs between the two sexes, as evidenced by morphological characteristics, such as body size and the tympanic membrane, which could result in sexual dimorphism in hearing sensitivity in fishes and frogs (Yan et al., 2000; van Dijk et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2016). However, other studies contradict the hypothesis of sexual dimorphism in hearing sensitivity (Caras et al., 2010; Schrode et al., 2014).

The match between the sensitivity of the auditory system and the energy spectrum of sender vocalizations influences the signal-to-noise ratio for receivers (Capranica and Moffat, 1983; Endler, 1992; Gerhardt and Schwartz, 2001). The so-called "match filter hypothesis" has been widely verified in fishes (Blaxter, 1981; Ladich and Yan, 1998), birds (Endler, 1992; Henry and Lucas, 2008), insects (Kostarakos et al., 2008), and frogs (Marquez and Bosch, 1997; Yu et al., 2006; Moreno-Gomez et al., 2013). However, recently studies found the match filter hypothesis was not always applicable in anurans (Gerhardt and Schwartz, 2001; Zhao et al., 2017). At present, no studies concerning hearing sensitivity and sexual differences in auditory sensitivity have been conducted in tropical island toads.

The black-spectacled toad Duttaphrynus melanostictus is a typical explosive breeder, with females larger than males (Ngo and Ngo, 2013), and males that produce mating calls (Wei et al., 2012). The species is widely distributed throughout Southeast Asia, with a range that spans Taiwan, southwestern and southern China (including Hainan Island), southward towards Indonesia, and westwards to India and Sri Lanka (Shieh, 1993; Fei et al., 2012). The acoustic properties of this species were found to vary in individuals from Thailand (Heyer, 1971), India (Hampson and Bennett, 2002), Myanmar (Wogan et al., 2003), and Indonesia (Marquez and Eekhout, 2006). In China, Wei et al. (2012) reported that the acoustic properties were highly variable across recording times, and analyzed the relationship between acoustic property and body size based on data from non-one by one individual under semi natural conditions.

In this study, we addressed the following questions: (1) whether acoustic properties reflect the body size of the individual under natural conditions; and (2) whether there is sexual dimorphism in peripheral auditory sensitivity in this species. Finally, we tested the match filter hypothesis in D. melanostictus.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

The experiment was conducted in an ecological park of Hainan Normal University (19Adeg 59' 54'' N, 110Adeg 20' 18.7'' E, elevation 35 m a.s.l.) in Hainan Province, China. Calls were recorded from 17 male D. melanostictus in November 2017. Nine females and 14 males were used in the auditory brainstem response (ABR) experiment. After the ABR experiments and individuals had recovered from anesthesia, they were returned to their original site within 24 h of being hand captured. The treatment procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Chengdu Institute of Biology, Chinese Academy of Sciences.

Vocalization recordings

The vocalizations of male D. melanostictus were recorded using a directional microphone (Sennheiser ME66 with K6 power module) connected to a digital audio recorder (Marantz PMD 661, 16-bit, 44.1 kHz) placed approximately 1 m from subjects, from 20:30 to 23:30, at ambient temperature 19-26AdegC, and relative humidity 75-90%.

Morphological data

Individuals were captured after calls were recorded to investigate possible relationships between vocalization characteristics and body size. Snout-vent length, head length, and head width were measured using a digital caliper (10810206, Berrylion, China).

Auditory brainstem response measurements

The experiment was conducted at the same study site range from Nov 2017 to Jan 2018. In total, 23 toads (14 males, 9 females) were used. We used noninvasive ABR to measure hearing in toads that were lightly anesthetized via water immersion for 8-11 min (males) or 18-24 min (females), using a 0.2% solution of MS-222 (Tricaine Methane Sulfonate). Individuals were then placed in a soundproof acoustic chamber (0.5 x 0.5 x 0.5 m) (Carey and Zelick, 1993; Zhang et al., 2012). When an individual showed no pain response to stimulation of the hind leg muscles using forceps, it was proceeded to the next stage of the experiment. The stimulus, ABR acquisition, equipment control, and data management procedures were similar to those in our previous studies (Zhang et al., 2012; Cui et al., 2017).

In brief, for each specimen, three 27-gauge stainless steel electrodes (Rochester Electro-Medical, Inc. FL, USA) were inserted subdermally at (1) the midline above the medulla (approximately 3 cm caudally to the snout), and (2) above the tympanum, and (3) in the ipsilateral front leg as inverting, noninverting, and ground electrodes, respectively. The recording electrodes were connected to a head stage and amplifier (PA4 and RA4, 20 gain, TDT) via wires wrapped in tin foil.

The stimulus was generated and the ABR was recorded using a digital signal processor RM2 (Tucker-Davis Technologies, Gainesville, USA), via fiber optic cables linked to RA4 and a USB linked to a laptop computer running custom software (Open ABR). Two types of stimuli, tone pips and clicks, were generated using Open ABR and delivered using a portable amplified field speaker (SME-AFS, Saul Miner of Electronic Inc., USA), which was driven by an RM2 and positioned on the table (at a height of 105 cm), approximately 110 cm in front of the individual's head. ABR recording stimulus levels were calibrated using a G.R.A.S. 46 BE A1/4-inch microphone (G.R.A.S. Sound and Vibration, Denmark, tianjia) with a CCP Supply (Type 12 AL, G.R.A.S. Sound and Vibration, Denmark) to a 60-dB sound pressure level (dB SPL re: 20 uPa) positioned at the individual's head. Stimuli were synthesized digitally at octave intervals from 0.6 kHz to 7 kHz, with a 1 ms rise/fall time, a 3 ms plateau time, and a sample rate of 24,414 Hz.

The authors recognize that the brief stimuli and short rise/fall times of the tone bursts used in this study were not ideal for accurately determining thresholds for low frequency sounds. However, increased rise/fall times and longer stimulus durations affect the brainstem response morphology. Given these constraints, we chose a 5 ms tone train with a 1 ms rise/fall time. All biological signals were notch-filtered at 50 Hz during data collection (Zhang et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2016; Cui et al., 2017).

The ABR thresholds were determined using methods similar to those in our previous studies (Zhang et al., 2012; Cui et al., 2017). Threshold measurements were defined as the lowest stimulus level for which no repeatable responses were recognized and were initiated at 90 dB SPL and reduced in 5 dB steps. We assumed that the 90-dB level was higher than all ABR thresholds of D. melanostictus for the stimuli used. ABRs were obtained from each individual at frequencies between 0.6 kHz and 7 kHz for ABR threshold estimation (Brittan-Powell et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2012).

Analysis and statistics

SPSS 19.0 (IBM SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for analysis. All data were examined for assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance using the Shapiro-Wilk and Levene's tests, respectively. Spontaneous vocalizations from 17 males were obtained from the recordings, and the acoustic properties (dominant frequency, fundamental frequency, note number, inter-note interval, note duration, and call duration) of the calls were analyzed. The sonograms of calls were prepared using Praat software (Boersma and Weeninkk, Version 5.1.11, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands). The files were analyzed and displayed using Adobe Audition 3.0 and Praat. The coefficient of variation (CV = SD/X*100%, where X is mean) for each acoustic property was computed separately to identify patterns of variation. Call properties were classified as static or dynamic based on inter-individual variation (CV) during bouts of calling (Gerhardt, 1991; Zhu et al., 2017).

If the CV of a given call property was less than 5%, it was identified as "static" as there was little variation between calls. Conversely, call properties were identified as "dynamic" when the CV was greater than 10%. Pearson correlation analysis was used to detect relationships between the call properties. ABR thresholds obtained from females and males in response to tone and click stimuli were sorted and analyzed using Independent samples t-tests. Results were expressed as mean +- SD, and P 0.05). The correlations between acoustic properties and body size are shown in Table II.

Table II.- Correlation analysis between acoustic properties and body size.

###Snout-vent###Head###Head

###length###length###width

Dominant frequency###0.01###0.07###0.02

Fundamental frequency###0.41###0.58###0.39

Note number###0.88###0.81###0.50

Inter-note interval###0.32###0.42###0.36

Note duration###0.94###0.01###0.09

Call duration###0.96###0.51###0.36

Sex differences in peripheral auditory sensitivity

Figure 3 shows a typical ABR response level series measured in one female and one male, for which thresholds of 40 dB SPL and 50 dB SPL were obtained, respectively. ABR thresholds are shown in Figure 4 (female, n = 9; male, n = 14). Figure 4 shows that the hearing frequency range was 0.6-2.0 kHz for females and 1.0-2.0 kHz for males. Remarkably, the best excitatory frequencies were 1.2-1.8 kHz in both females and males. Compared with males, the ABR thresholds in females were significantly smaller at 0.6-2.4 kHz (P <0.05).

Relationship between hearing sensitivity and vocalization characteristics

The ABR thresholds were lowest at a frequency of 1.8 kHz (42.2 +- 2.6 dB) (Fig. 5). The main energy spectrum of male vocalizations was 1.0-2.0 kHz, during which both male and female D. melanostictus showed sensitive auditory perception. In general, the spectral structures of male vocalizations matched both male and female hearing sensitivity.

DISCUSSION

In anurans, classifying the diversification of calls is important for understanding their behavior and the evolution of acoustic communication (Krishna and Bosch, 2007). Sexual selection produces individuals that emit calls with longer call duration and more notes (Ryan and Keddy-Hector, 1992; Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Gridi-Papp et al., 2006). The male D. melanostictus emit a series of multisyllabic calls which are composed of single notes with dominant frequency 1494 +- 80 Hz. These results were similar to those from another study of this species in Zhejiang Province, China (Wei et al., 2012), which showed a dominant frequency of about 1.5 kHz in males from the Bali population, Republic of Indonesia (Marquez and Eekhout, 2006).

However, they were different from results of males in Bangkok that have a dominant frequency of 1.0-1.7 kHz (Heyer, 1971), and those in the Coorg population of Karnataka that have an average dominant frequency of 1.6 kHz (Hampson and Bennett, 2002), and those with a frequency over 3.0 kHz in Burma (Wogan et al., 2003). Our results suggest that males compete by increasing the number of notes to make their acoustic signal more complex, and that the dominant frequency represents geographic variation in complexity. Geographic variation in anuran calls was also reported in Leptodactylus fuscus (Heyer and Reid, 2003), Colostethus palmatus (Bernal et al., 2005), and Dendropsophus cruzi (Tessarolo et al., 2016).

As previously reported in Hyla labialis (Gutierrez and Luddecke, 2002), Rhacophorus dennysi (Wang et al., 2012), and Philautus odontotarsus (Zhu et al., 2017), the acoustic properties could also have reflected the body size of D. melanostictus. Furthermore, the dominant frequency was classified as static, suggesting it may be under stabilizing selection and is probably used in species recognition. However, the note and call durations were dynamic, suggesting they may be under directional selection and more likely used in mate choice (Gerhardt, 1991; Zhu et al., 2017).

Studies have verified that hearing sensitivity at all frequencies within the hearing range can be influenced by sex, and also influence thresholds in a stimulus-specific manner (Miranda and Wilczynski, 2009; Shen et al., 2011). There was a significant sex difference in peripheral auditory sensitivity in D. melanostictus-females had more sensitive hearing than males. However, this conclusion contradicts those from concave-eared torrent frogs, Odorrana tormota (Shen et al., 2011) and large odorous frogs, O. graminea (Liu et al., 2014), in which males are more sensitive than females to ultrasound. Furthermore, in American bullfrogs, the males are more sensitive than females to low frequencies (Werner et al., 2009). Although these studies suggest sex-related differences in hearing sensitivity is species specific, the dimorphism in peripheral auditory sensitivity may result from differences in the breeding roles of the sexes.

Interesting, our previous work suggested female choice plays a major role in shaping the mating behavior of this tropical toads (Wang et al., 2018).

The match between male acoustic signals and female hearing abilities is a common feature in call-producing and hearing species (Ladich and Yan, 1998; Henry and Lucas, 2008; Kostarakos et al., 2008; Moreno-Gomez et al., 2013); however, there are studies that contradict this (Gerhardt and Schwartz, 2001; Wright et al., 2003; Zhao et al., 2017). The match is thought to have arisen over evolutionary time, as it promotes conspecific information transfer and reduces interference from natural environments (Moreno-Gomez et al., 2013). In this study, we found that the tuning of female and male hearing sensitivity was well matched with the frequencies of male calls. This result clarified two aspects: for efficient communication in mixed-species choruses, the frequencies used in vocalizations and hearing was matched in each anuran species, and successful reproduction requires that females detect, recognize, and localize the vocalizations of a conspecific male (Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Nityananda and Bee, 2011).

Moreover, in anurans, the auditory receiving systems are thought to have coevolved with acoustic sender signals specifically (Ryan and Wilczynski, 1988; Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Witte et al., 2005). The match between frequencies used in hearing and vocalizations in D. melanostictus suggested that they have co-evolved. Sexual selection produces males that produce more complex calls, and female evolved selective pressure lead to improved acoustic communication (Hetherington, 1994; Feng et al., 2006).

In conclusion, our results show that male D. melanostictus emit complex multisyllabic calls by increasing the number of notes. Dominant frequency and note duration could reflect body size in this species. The sexual dimorphism in peripheral auditory sensitivity, and the spectral structures of male vocalizations matched both male and female hearing sensitivity. Although receiving systems and acoustic communication signals are thought to have co-evolved, sexual dimorphism in the peripheral auditory sensitivity was observed. This suggested that female choice may play a more important role than male-male competition during breeding of this species.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Chun Hua Zhou and Xin Tong Li for assistance with fieldwork. We also thank two anonymous reviewers and our handling editor for valuable criticism that has greatly improved this manuscript.

Statement of conflict of interest

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

REFERENCES

Bee, M.A., Perrill, S.A. and Owen, P.C., 1999. Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana clamitans). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 45: 177-184. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050551

Bernal, X.E., Guarnizo, C. and Luddecke, H., 2005. Geographic variation in advertisement call and genetic structure of Colostethus palmatus (Anura, Dendrobatidae) from the Colombian Andes. Herpetologica, 61: 395-408. https://doi.org/10.1655/04-87.1

Blaxter, J.H.S., 1981. The swimbladder and hearing. In: Hearing and sound communication in fishes (eds. W.N. Tavolga, A.N. Popper and R.R. Fay). Springer Berlin Heidelberg, New York, pp. 61-71. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7186-5_3

Brittan-Powell, E.F., Christensen-Dalsgaard, J., Tang, Y., Carr, C. and Dooling, R.J., 2010. The auditory brainstem response in two lizard species. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 128: 787-794. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3458813

Capranica, R.R. and Moffat, A.J.M., 1983. Neurobehavioral correlates of sound communication in anurans. In: Advances in vertebrate neuroethology (eds. J.P. Ewert, R.R. Capranica and D.J. Ingle). Plenum, USA, pp. 701-730. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-4412-4_36

Caras, M.L., Brenowitz, E. and Rubel, E.W., 2010. Peripheral auditory processing changes seasonally in Gambel's white-crowned sparrow. J. Comp. Physiol. A, 196: 581-599. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-010-0545-1

Carey, M.B. and Zelick, R., 1993. The effect of sound level, temperature and dehydration on the brainstem auditory evoked potential in anuran amphibians. Hear. Res., 70: 216-228. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-5955(93)90160-3

Cocroft, R.B. and Ryan, M.J., 1995. Patterns of advertisement call evolution in toads and chorus frogs. Anim. Behav., 49: 283-303. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1995.0043

Cui, J.G., Tang, Y.Z. and Narins, P.M., 2012. Real estate ads in Emei music frog vocalizations: Female preference for calls emanating from burrows. Biol. Lett., 8: 337-340. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.1091

Cui, J.G., Zhu, B.C., Fang, G.Z., Smith, E., Brauth, S.E. and Tang, Y.Z., 2017. Effect of the level of anesthesia on the auditory brainstem response in the Emei music frog (Babina daunchina). PLoS One, 12: e0169449. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169449

Davies, N.B. and Halliday, T.R., 1978. Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature, 274: 683-685. https://doi.org/10.1038/274683a0

Endler, J.A., 1992. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am. Natural., 139: S125-S153. https://doi.org/10.1086/285308

Fei, L., Ye, C.Y. and Jiang, J.P., 2012. Colored atlas of Chinese amphibians and their distributions. Science and Technology Press, Sichuan.

Feng, A.S., Narins, P.M., Xu, C.H., Lin, W.Y., Yu, Z.L., Qiu, Q., Xu, Z.M. and Shen, J.X., 2006. Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature, 440: 333-336. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04416

Forstmeier, W., Nakagawa, S., Grifth, S.C. and Kempenaers, B., 2014. Female extra-pair mating: Adaptation or genetic constraint? Trends Ecol. Evol., 29: 456-464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.005

Gall, M.D., Brierley, L.E. and Lucas, J.R., 2011. Species and sex effects on auditory processing in brown-headed cowbirds and red-winged blackbirds. Anim. Behav., 81: 973-982. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.032

Gerhardt, H.C., 1991. Female mate choice in treefrogs: Static and dynamic acoustic criteria. Anim. Behav., 42: 615-635. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80245-3

Gerhardt, H.C. and Huber, F., 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. Chicago University Press, Chicago.

Gerhardt, H.C. and Schwartz, J.J., 2001. Auditory tuning and frequency preferences in anurans. In: Anuran communication (ed. M.J. Ryan). Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC, pp. 73-85.

Gridi-Papp, M., Rand, A.S. and Ryan, M.J., 2006. Animal communication: Complex call production in the Tungara frog. Nature, 441: 38-38. https://doi.org/10.1038/441038a

Gutierrez, G. and Luddecke, H., 2002. Mating pattern and hatching success in a population of the Andean frog Hyla labialis. Amphibia-Reptilia, 23: 281-292. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685380260449162

Hampson, K. and Bennett, D., 2002. Advertisement calls of amphibians at Lackunda Estate, Coorg, Karnataka. In: Frogs of Coorg, Karnataka, India (ed. D. Bennett). Viper Press, Glossop, pp. 115-136.

Henry, K.S. and Lucas, J.R., 2008. Coevolution of auditory sensitivity and temporal resolution with acoustic signal space in three songbirds. Anim. Behav., 76: 1659-1671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.003

Hetherington, T.E., 1994. Sexual differences in the tympanic frequency responses of the American bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana). J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 96: 1186-1188. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.410326

Heyer, W.R., 1971. Mating calls of some frogs from Thailand. Fieldiana (Zool.), 58: 61-82.

Heyer, W.R. and Reid, Y.R., 2003. Does advertisement call variation coincide with genetic variation in the genetically diverse frog taxon currently known as Leptodactylus fuscus (Amphibia: Leptodactylidae)? Anais Acad. Brasil. Cienc., 75: 39-54. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0001-37652003000100006

Kostarakos, K., Hartbauer, M. and Romer, H., 2008. Matched filters, mate choice and the evolution of sexually selected traits. PLoS One, 3: e3005. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003005

Krishna, S.N. and Bosch, J., 2007. Breeding behaviour and advertisement calls of the tree-hole breeding frog Ramanella montana (Microhylidae) in the Western Ghats, S. India. Acta zool. Sin., 53: 575-578.

Ladich, F. and Yan, H.Y., 1998. Correlation between auditory sensitivity and vocalization in anabantoid fishes. J. comp. Physiol. A, 182: 737-746. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050218

Liu, W.R., Shen, J.X., Zhang, Y.J., Xu, Z.M., Qi, Z. and Xue, M.Q., 2014. Auditory sexual difference in the large odorous frog Odorrana graminea. J. comp. Physiol. A, 200: 311-316. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0885-3

Marquez, R. and Bosch, J., 1997. Male advertisement call and female preference in sympatric and allopatric midwife toads. Anim. Behav., 54: 1333-1345. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0529

Marquez, R. and Eekhout, X.R., 2006. Advertisement calls of six species of anurans from Bali, Republic of Indonesia. J. nat. Hist., 40: 571-588. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222930600712129

Marquez, R., Penna, M., Marques, P. and do Amaral, J.P.S., 2005. Diverse types of advertisement calls in the frogs Eupsophus calcaratus and E. roseus (Leptodactylidae): A quantitative comparison. Herpetol. J., 15: 257-263.

Miranda, J.A. and Wilczynski, W., 2009. Sex differences and androgen influences on midbrain auditory thresholds in the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea. Hear. Res., 252: 79-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.004

Moreno-Gomez, F.N., Sueur, J., Soto-Gamboa, M. and Penna, M., 2013. Female frog auditory sensitivity, male calls, and background noise: Potential influences on the evolution of a peculiar matched filter. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., 110: 814-827. https://doi.org/10.1111/bij.12156

Narins, P.M. and Capranica, R.R., 1976. Sexual differences in the auditory system of the tree frog Eleutherodactylus coqui. Science, 192: 378-380. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1257772

Ngo, B.V. and Ngo, C.D., 2013. Reproductive activity and advertisement calls of the Asian common toad Duttaphrynus melanostictus (Amphibia, Anura, Bufonidae) from Bach Ma National Park, Vietnam. Zool. Stud., 52: 47-59. https://doi.org/10.1186/1810-522X-52-12

Nityananda, V. and Bee, M.A., 2011. Finding your mate at a cocktail party: Frequency separation promotes auditory stream segregation of concurrent voices in multi-species frog choruses. PLoS One, 6: e21191. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021191

Penna, M., 2004. Amplification and spectral shifts of vocalizations inside burrows of the frog Eupsophus calcaratus (Leptodactylidae). J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 116: 1254-1260. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1768257

Ron, S.R., 2008. The evolution of female mate choice for complex calls in Tungara frogs. Anim. Behav., 76: 1783-1794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024

Ryan, M.J., 1998. Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences. Science, 281: 1999-2003. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.281.5385.1999

Ryan, M.J. and Keddy-Hector, A., 1992. Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases. Am. Natural., 139: S4-S35. https://doi.org/10.1086/285303

Ryan, M.J. and Wilczynski, W., 1988. Coevolution of sender and receiver: Effect on local mate preference in cricket frogs. Science, 240: 1786-1788. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.240.4860.1786

Schrode, K.M., Buerkle, N.P., Brittan-Powell, E.F. and Bee, M.A., 2014. Auditory brainstem responses in Cope's gray treefrog (Hyla chrysoscelis): Effects of frequency, level, sex and size. J. comp. Physiol. A, 200: 221-238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0880-8

Shen, J.X., Xu, Z.M., Yu, Z.L., Wang, S., Zheng, D.Z. and Fan, S.C., 2011. Ultrasonic frogs show extraordinary sex differences in auditory frequency sensitivity. Nat. Commun., 2: 342-347. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1339

Shieh, J., 1993. The breeding ecology of Bufo melanostitus. MS thesis, Tunghai University, Taiwan, ROC.

Sun, J.W.C. and Narins, P.M., 2005. Anthropogenic sounds differentially affect amphibian call rate. Biol. Conserv., 121: 419-427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2004.05.017

Tarano, Z., 2001. Variation in male advertisement calls in the Neotropical frog Physalaemus enesefae. Copeia, 4:1064-1072. https://doi.org/10.1643/0045-8511(2001)001[1064:VIMACI]2.0.CO;2

Tessarolo, G., Maciel, N.M., Ribeiro, Morais, A. and Bastos, R.P., 2016. Geographic variation in advertisement calls among populations of Dendropsophus cruzi (Anura: Hylidae). Herpetol. J., 26: 219-227.

Van-Dijk, P., Mason, M.J. and Narins, P.M., 2002. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions in frogs: Correlation with middle and inner ear properties. Hear. Res., 173: 100-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00605-6

Wang, J.C., Cui, J.G., Shi, H.T., Brauth, S.E. and Tang, Y.Z., 2012. Effects of body size and environmental factors on the acoustic structure and temporal rhythm of calls in Rhacophorus dennysi. Asian Herpetol. Res., 3: 205-212. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1245.2012.00205

Wang, J.C., Wang, T.L., Fu, S.H., Brauth, S.E. and Cui, J.G., 2016. Auditory brainstem responses in the Chinese tiger frog Hoplobatrachus chinensis (Osbeck, 1765) (Anura: Dicroglossidae) reveal sexually dimorphic hearing sensitivity. Ital. J. Zool., 83: 482-489. https://doi.org/10.1080/11250003.2016.1222638

Wang, T.L., Jia, L.L., Zhai, X.F., Cui, J.G. and Wang, J.C., 2018. Atypical assortative mating based on body size in an explosive-breeding toad from a tropical island of southern China. Behav. Process., 151: 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.02.018

Wei, L., Zhao, L.H., Ma, X.H., Fan, X.L., Ma, X.M. and Lin, Z.H., 2012. Advertisement call variability in the Black-spined toad Bufo melanostictus (Anura: Bufonidae) during the breeding season in Lishui, Zhejiang, China. Asian Herpetol. Res., 3: 157-162. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1245.2012.00157

Werner, Y.L., Pylka, J., Schneider, H., Seifan, M., Walkowiak, W. and Werner-Reiss, U., 2009. Function of the sexually dimorphic ear of the American bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana: brief review and new insight. J. exp. Biol., 212: 2204-2214. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.027516

Witte, K., Farris, H.E., Ryan, M.J. and Wilczynski, W., 2005. How cricket frog females deal with a noisy world: Habitat-related differences in auditory tuning. Behav. Ecol., 16: 571-579. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ari032

Wogan, G.O.U., Win, H., Thin, T., Lwin, K.S., Shein, A.K., Kyi, S.W. and Tun, H., 2003. A new species of Bufo (Anura: Bufonidae) from Myanmar (Burma), and redescription of the little-known species Bufo stuarti Smith 1929. Proc. California Acad. Sci., 54: 141-153.

Wright, T.F., Cortopassi, K.A., Bradbury, J.W. and Dooling, R.J., 2003. Hearing and vocalizations in the orange-fronted conure (Aratinga canicularis). J. comp. Psychol., 117: 87-95. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.87

Yan, H.Y., Fine, M.L., Horn, N.S. and Colon, W.E., 2000. Variability in the role of the gasbladder in fish audition. J. comp. Physiol. A, 186: 435-445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050443

Yu, Z.L., Qiu, Q., Xu, Z.M. and Shen, J.X., 2006. Auditory response characteristics of the piebald odorous frog and their implications. J. comp. Physiol. A, 192: 801-806. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-006-0125-6

Zhang, D., Cui, J.G. and Tang, Y.Z., 2012. Plasticity of peripheral auditory frequency sensitivity in Emei music frog. PLoS One, 7: e45792. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045792

Zhao, L.H., Wang, J.C., Yang, Y., Zhu, B.C., Brauth, S.E., Tang, Y.Z. and Cui, J.G., 2017. An exception to the matched filter hypothesis: A mismatch of male call frequency and female best hearing frequency in a torrent frog. Ecol. Evol., 7: 419-428. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.2621

Zhu, B.C., Wang, J.C., Brauth, S.E., Tang, Y.Z. and Cui, J.G., 2017. The spectral structure of vocalizations match hearing sensitivity but imprecisely in Philautus odontotarsus. Bioacoustics, 26: 121-134. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2016.1221778
COPYRIGHT 2019 Knowledge Bylanes
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2019 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Wang, Tongliang; Jia, Lele; Zhai, Xiaofei; Cui, Jianguo; Wang, Jichao
Publication:Pakistan Journal of Zoology
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:9CHIN
Date:Apr 30, 2019
Words:5139
Previous Article:Effect of Phytase Supplementation on Growth Performance in Broiler Chickens.
Next Article:mir-16-5p as a Suitable Reference Gene for Normalization of Quantitative Real Time PCR in Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2021 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters