Printer Friendly

The 'math gap': puzzling sex differences.

The 'math gap': Puzzling sex differences

For some time, scientists have observed that boys score much higher than girls on mathematics tests. Based on studies of more than 100,000 intellectually gifted 12- and 13-year-olds, for example, there are 13 boys for every girl who scores at least 700 out of 800 on the mathematics section of the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) (SN: 4/27/85, p.263). What researchers have not been able to calculate, however, is how the "math gap" is affected by parental attitudes and cultural values.

Now, a report in the November DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY says that several plausible environmental influences on mathematics achievement do not appear to sway the SAT scores of gifted and above-average junior-high students. The first possibility examined by Camilla Benbow Of Iowa State University in Ames and Cindy L. Raymond of Yale University was that youngsters consider mathematically related fields to be masculine domains, so girls are less motivated in those areas. Second, since no sex differences in verbal ability have been found, the psychologists looked at whether parents encouraged mathematically talented students more than verbally talented students and whether males received more encouragement in quantitative areas than females.

"It is improbable that these factors influence the sex differences in math achievement among intellectually talented children," says Benbow. The data, she adds, do not apply to normal-range achievers in mathematics.

The 200 extremely talented students in the study, whose average age was nearly 14 years old, had taken the SAT about one year earlier and scored at least 700 on the mathematics section or at least 630 on the verbal section. Youngsters who surpassed both the mathematics and verbal cutoffs were excluded from the project.

A slightly older comparison group of 111 students had taken the SAT about two years prior to the study and achieved approximately chance scores (combined mathematics and verbal scores no greater than 540). They had, however, obtained extremely high scores on an ingrade achievement test. The SAT is designed to be taken by high school seniors.

On questionnaires, neither the gifted nor the comparison group reported significant gender differences in parental encouragement. Intellectually talented males did not perceive greater mathematical encouragement than did their female counterparts. Fathers were not more involved with mathematically talented children than with verbally talented ones, and the reverse was not reported for mothers. Fathers were reported to be somewhat more math-oriented and mothers more verbal-oriented, but these tendencies were not strong.

Furthermore, 59 percent of the gifted students and nearly three-quarters of the comparison group did not label mathematically related areas as masculine and verbal areas as feminine. The tendency to assign one sex preeminence in an academic area was not related either to the pattern of parental encouragement or to SAT scores.

These two factors -- parental attitudes and the perception of mathematics as masculine -- join a growing list of environmental variables that have failed to account for sex differences in mathematics achievement, notes Benbow. She has, for instance, uncovered no sex differences in childhood toy preferences of the same students. This undermines the contention that boys prefer playthings, such as construction toys and building blocks, that promote mathematical reasoning abilities. Researchers have also observed no sex differences among gifted students in assessments of the future career value of mathematics and mathematics course-taking in high school.

Despite these results, Benbow says, "I certainly believe the environment is involved somehow in these well-established sex differences." Future research, she says, should examine whether girls have a wider variety of interests than boys and thus have less time to pursue mathematics.

On the other hand, says Benbow, biological traits such as left-handedness and susceptibility to allergies are associated with mathematical and erbal precocity. She suggests these traits may be fostered in part by overexposure of a fetus to the hormone testosterone, which some scientists say enhances the brain's right-hemisphere development and improves communication between hemispheres; this, in turn, may aid in the comprehension of mathemaatics and relationships between difficult words.

"No one knows when critical math and verbal abilities first develop," says Benbow, "but environment and biology can't really be separated."
COPYRIGHT 1986 Science Service, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1986, Gale Group. All rights reserved. Gale Group is a Thomson Corporation Company.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:research on why boys score higher than girls on mathematics tests
Author:Bower, Bruce
Publication:Science News
Date:Dec 6, 1986
Words:686
Previous Article:How plants say 'no' to fungus.
Next Article:Coming: a drug to sober you up?
Topics:


Related Articles
Gender differences in young adolescents' mathematics and science achievement.
School effectiveness of a streamed-school system: a multilevel modelling of the Hong Kong secondary schools.
TIMSS: gender inequities still exist. [Third International Mathematics and Science Study] .
Teachers have the power to alleviate math anxiety.
Denker done as South's girls basketball coach.
PREPWEEK BRIEFLY.
3 incumbents retain seats on school boards.
Women aren't "small men": women's health issues are different than men's and need to be addressed specifically.
Egg shell game: chicks' sex isn't just a matter of chance.

Terms of use | Copyright © 2016 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters