Printer Friendly

Plasticity-mediated persistence in new and changing environments.

1. A Brief History of the Baldwin Effect

Although writers in antiquity recognized that the environment could influence an organism's appearance [1], this did not receive theoretical consideration until Lamarck [2]. For Lamarck, adaptation was the result of the interplay between development and the environment. Phenotypes modified by the environment could then be passed on to offspring. Darwin, in The Origin of Species [3], although not denying the possibility of the inheritance of acquired characters [4], focussed on adaptation in a way that downplayed the significance of environmental modifications. With selection acting on chance variations, adaptation became an infinitesimally slow intergenerational process rather than a product of development. The apparent unimportance of environmental modification was confirmed by Johannsen [5] who showed that selection of extreme phenotypes in inbred lines did not result in evolutionary change. By the beginning of the 1900s, Neo-Darwinists and Neo-Lamarckians were divided on the importance of environmental modifications in evolution. However, there were problems with both positions. The Neo-Lamarckians pointed out the difficulty of explaining Neo-Darwinian adaptation in a new environment, since the chance production of fortuitous variation seemed implausible, while the slow pace of adaptation could not match the rapidity of environmental change [6]. On the other hand, no less an authority than Romanes had disavowed most of the evidence for the inheritance of acquired characters [7]. It was into this context that a "not-quite-third" position, that of the Organicists (see Appendix A for glossary), was developed, with a synthesis of positions articulated by Baldwin [6]. The so-called Baldwin effect [8] has proven to be both prescient (Appendix B) and misunderstood (Appendix C). Because of this, it is worth briefly discussing the Organicist position, before highlighting modern evidence for the significance of plasticity in new and changing environments.

Before discussing the Organicist position, it is important to recognize what this position was not: it was not a compromise between Neo-Lamarckism and Neo-Darwinism. It brought Lamarck's first law of use/disuse (and other forms of plasticity) firmly in the camp of Neo-Darwinism, but gutted Neo-Lamarckism in the process (Poulton, quoted in appendix of [6]). In the points that follow, the primary role of natural selection will be readily apparent; Baldwin's organic selection was a supplement to, not a replacement of, natural selection.

The Baldwin effect can be divided into several stages [6]. (1) Organisms are exposed to a novel environment. (2) If all individuals produce unfit phenotypes in the new environment, the population will go extinct. (3) Any organisms that can plastically alter their phenotypes to meet the challenges of the new environment will survive. (4) Natural selection will thus weed out nonplastic or maladaptively plastic individuals, while adaptively plastic individuals will survive and reproduce. (5) The plastic modifications made in the parental generation will not be passed on to their offspring. (6) Plastic individuals will make up a larger proportion of the offspring than in the parental generation. Therefore, many offspring will have the capacity to modify their phenotypes in the same manner as their successful parents. It will look as if they had inherited acquired characters, when in fact they had inherited genetically based plasticity. (7) There is now time for genetic variation to arise to improve the fit between the population and the environment. This can occur in at least one of four ways: (a) coincident variation can occur. Variations arise randomly in the population. Some of these variants may reduce plasticity such that the plastically modified phenotype is negatively affected. These variants will perform poorly and will be weeded out by selection. Other variants will arise that reduce plasticity in the direction of the plastic modification. These variants will be favoured and will spread through the population. Thus, although the phenotype itself appears the same from generation to generation, it becomes correspondingly less plastic, until a change in environment will no longer induce a change in phenotype. These coincident variants would be particularly likely to be favoured if there was some cost to plasticity. For instance, learning through imitation could permit offspring to resemble their parents, but the time it takes to learn could be prohibitive, and thus an instinct could evolve through the imitated phenotype becoming genetically assimilated. Steps (1) through 7(a) are generally what is referred to as the Baldwin effect ([8], but see Appendix C), which was Baldwin's primary means of differentiating the Organicists from other perspectives (Figure 1).

Baldwin allowed for other non-mutually exclusive scenarios to play out. 7(b) Variants could arise that increase plasticity, thereby furthering the fit between organism and environment and furthering the extent to which evolution could be directed (as in (a)). (c) Plasticity could, however, increase to the point that genetic change became prohibitive. As a psychologist Baldwin focussed on learning and argued that genetic inheritance could give way to social inheritance brought about by particularly plastic learners. (d) Correlated variants could occur that improve the modified phenotype or other phenotypes permitted to survive by plasticity (in particular, phenotypes that would have contributed to population demise in the absence of plasticity, or phenotypes that could supplant the need for plasticity--see Appendix C for an example of this latter possibility).

Collectively, Baldwin referred to stages (1)-(7) as organic selection, and stage 7(a)-(d) as orthoplasy, or the "directive determination" of evolution through organic selection. For simplicity I will refer to stages (1)-(6) as plasticity-mediated persistence (PMP), while stage 7(a)-(c) involves the numerous possibilities of plasticity evolution (changes to slope and/or elevation of the reaction norm). 7(a) is commonly referred to as genetic assimilation, and 7(a)-(c) as genetic accommodation [9]. 7(d) may, but does not have to, involve genetic accommodation. It is worth pointing out that, for Baldwin, plasticity was largely an adaptation in its own right, a consequence of natural selection, and so maladaptive plasticity and its consequences (such as genetic compensation, [10]) were not envisioned by him.

Now that we have revisited the groundbreaking work of the Organicists, it is time to discuss the evidence for the integral role of PMP in adaptation to novel environments. Evidence for the association between plasticity and persistence will first be reviewed, and then some conceptual and methodological issues that require further attention will be discussed.

2. Evidence for Plasticity-Mediated Persistence

The first step of the Baldwin effect involves the persistence of individuals and populations in novel environments, with such persistence attributable to phenotypic plasticity (Figure 2). PMP is satisfied under the following conditions. (1) No evolution: all individuals produce optimal plastic responses in the new environment. (2) Persistence awaiting evolutionary rescue: all individuals produce suboptimal plastic responses in the new environment that permit persistence long enough for more fit genotypes to arise and spread. (3) Selection on standing genetic variation: some individuals are adaptively plastic and are favoured in the new environment. PMP does not occur if plasticity evolves from de novo mutations after colonization. A wealth of empirical and theoretical evidence has accumulated that suggests or directly demonstrates PMP in natural populations, particularly from studies on evolutionary rescue during climate change, work on invasive species and colonization success, and a disparate group of experiments on natural populations.

2.1. Evolutionary Rescue and Climate Change. When populations encounter novel environments through dispersal or climate change, genotypes that were fit in the old environment may no longer be fit in the new environment (Figure 2). The time it takes for more fit genotypes to arise may be prohibitively long, resulting in population extinction. Yet, successful colonization via adaptive evolution does occur, a scenario known as evolutionary rescue (ER) [11,12]. ER can take the form of de novo mutations [10], standing genetic variation [13], or the introduction of adaptive alleles from migrants [14]. The interaction between ER and plasticity has only recently been addressed [11].

ER may not be possible in coarse-grained environments [15]. Plasticity, by slowing the rate of population decline, can overcome this hurdle, providing time for beneficial mutations to arise [16]. This has been confirmed empirically: the likelihood of extinction for great tits increased 500-fold in the absence of plastic responses to climate change [17]. This was largely true in thirteen other bird species, although faster generation times offset the need for plasticity [17]. Climate change has resulted in population declines of numerous nonplastic species, suggesting that ER does not have time to work in the absence of plasticity [18,19]. For instance, rising temperatures shifted flowering time but not West Greenland caribou calving time, producing a trophic mismatch that declined calf production fourfold [20]. Species that can plastically maintain synchrony with peak food production have shown no such population decline [21, 22]. However, there is no consistent rule. Costs and limits of plasticity can constrain plastic responses, resulting in higher fitness for populations that do not plastically improve synchrony [23, 24], while ER can operate to improve synchrony in the absence of plasticity [25]. The above evidence therefore supports the prediction that plasticity is sufficient but not necessary to promote persistence, and that ER can occur in the absence of plasticity.

2.2. Invasive Success. Invasion is a three-step process: dispersal, establishment, and range expansion [26]. Factors besides plasticity have been identified in invasive success (e.g., [2729]), but plasticity may also play a role.

2.2.1. Dispersal. In order to successfully invade a new environment, individuals must first disperse to that environment. PMP predicts that species with high dispersal rates should also be highly plastic, as dispersal involves encountering spatial heterogeneity [30]. Indeed, dispersal of nonplastic organisms can reduce the likelihood of successful colonization by introducing maladaptive alleles to colonizing populations [31]. Hollander [32] tested the relationship between dispersal and plasticity in 258 species of marine invertebrates and found that on average dispersing species were more plastic than nondispersing species. Presumably, without such plasticity dispersers would fail to colonize the locations to which they disperse. Modelling work has shown that the relationship between the period of environmental sensitivity and the timing of dispersal matters: if plastic modifications to the organism occur irreversibly before dispersal, colonization success is low; if it occurs after dispersal, colonization success is much higher [33]. However, it is clear that many species have high dispersal rates and low levels of plasticity, so plasticity is again sufficient but not necessary for colonization [32]. Rapid generation times, for instance, may allow colonizing populations to rapidly evolve to meet the demands of the new environment [17].

Dispersal itself can be plastic in a manner that inhibits or improves invasive success. Traits associated with dispersal can be plastic in a way that maximizes dispersal within a habitat but limits the possibility of encountering new habitats [34]. On the other hand, dispersal can increase under stress, exposing offspring to novel environments [35]. Invasive species may therefore exhibit higher dispersal immediately upon colonization of stressful environments, simultaneously expanding their range [26] while decreasing their probability of establishment through a reduction in population size [33]. So plasticity in dispersal rate may inhibit persistence.

2.2.2. Establishment and Range Expansion. Invasive species often display a suite of functionally relevant plastic modifications upon encountering a new environment (e.g., [3638]), but it is not always clear if this plasticity is necessary for persistence or if, in the absence of plasticity, invasive success would still be high. For instance, Lande [39] showed that preexistent plasticity could rapidly evolve during colonization. This model received empirical support through the repeated evolution of increased plasticity for ion-motive enzyme activity during multiple colonization events of freshwater by a marine copepod, likely through repeated selection on preexisting standing genetic variation [40]. But this only shows that plasticity was beneficial, not that it was necessary for colonization. A model by Thibert-Plante and Hendry [33] deserves empirical support, as it demonstrates a 40% colonization success rate for nonplastic individuals but an 87% colonization success rate for plastic individuals, with the importance of plasticity increasing with the strength of selection. This suggests that plasticity is at least sometimes required for establishment.

The PMP hypothesis has led to predictions about the degree of plasticity that should be evident in invasive species [41]: (1) invasive species should be plastic for relevant adaptive traits; (2) invasive species should be as or more plastic than their ancestors; (3) invasive species should be more plastic than the species they displace; (4) plasticity should be greater in invasive species than in noninvasive species. The last year alone has seen numerous studies testing these predictions (Table 1), while several reviews discuss the current state of the matter (Table 2). A related prediction, that plastic species should have greater geographic ranges, has received limited empirical support [42-45].

The most common research program simply measures plasticity in invasive species and concludes that such plasticity may have contributed to invasive success. Although interesting, this tells us little about PMP. It is usually unknown if plasticity evolved in the new environment or preexisted prior to invasion. Comparison between populations of invasive species from their native and introduced ranges helps address this question--if plasticity is similarly present in both forms, it suggests the preadaptive significance of plasticity [46], while if plasticity is higher in the invading populations it suggests that plasticity was imperfect but adaptive [39]. Resurrection studies, where seeds from plants collected During the initial phase of invasion a regrown with seeds collected generations later, have confirmed that plasticity often increases during invasion [47,48]. Such studies are still not ideal, as they do not address whether plasticity itself was essential for invasive success.

One way to address PMP is to compare plasticity between invasive species and their native competitors. If plasticity is essential for colonization, theory suggests that invasive species will be more plastic than the species they displace. If plasticity is unessential, there will be no such trend. Many individual comparisons have been conducted with equivocal results. Meta-analyses, however, support PMP (Table 2): although there is variation among comparisons, invasive species are on average more plastic than their competitors [49,50]. Such comparisons, however, suffer from interpretive problems. Invasive species may only need to be as plastic as their competitors in order to meet the challenges of a new environment ("join-the-local" hypothesis [51]). More alarmingly, these studies are usually performed under isolation and for contrived phenotypes and environments [52], but the extent and significance of plasticity for invasive success can only be assessed under natural conditions with competition. An ideal experiment was conducted by Engel and Tollrian [53]. Invasive and native species of Daphnia were raised in the presence and absence of predators, separately and while competing. The nonplastic native species outperformed the plastic invader while competing in the absence of predators, but morphological plasticity in the invader gave the invader a competitive edge under predation. In this case clear predictions about the significance of plasticity could be made and assessed; for many phenotypes measured in invasive species the significance of plasticity is less clear.

A second way to establish PMP is to compare plasticity between invasive species and noninvasive conspecifics that have been introduced to but did not become established in new environments. (Note that some studies compare invasive species and "noninvasive" species, where the noninvasive species is simply the species being displaced by the invaders. These studies address prediction number 3 above and are prone to its limitations (see, e.g., [54]).) Invasive birds and mammals tend to have greater plasticity in learning than their noninvasive conspecifics (Appendix D). One particularly interesting study compared invasive and noninvasive populations of the same species. In the plant Centaurea stoebe, both diploid and tetraploid populations occur across its natural European range, but in North America tetraploids have been the only successful invaders. This is despite numerous opportunities for diploid establishment. A comparison in plasticity of physiological and life-history traits in all three groups showed greater plasticity in tetraploids, but no difference in plasticity between American and European tetraploids. The direction of plasticity appeared to be adaptive under North American climatic conditions, suggesting plasticity's important role in invasive success in this species [55].

2.2.3. Plasticity in Response to Invaders. Introduced species provide a new environment to which native species can respond. Plastic responses in the native species may facilitate persistence in the face of such introductions. For instance, behavioural plasticity upon exposure to an introduced predator may prevent the collapse of the prey population [36, 56, 57]. Plasticity in the native species may even prevent successful invasion of the introduced species [58]. On the other hand, inappropriate plastic responses to invaders may inhibit persistence [59].

2.3. Other Evidence. Studies from common garden experiments have provided some evidence for PMP. For instance, genotypes of Impatiens capensis that inhabit distinct environments and which differ in their degree of plasticity were transplanted into the respective genotypes habitat. The plastic genotype performed better in the nonplastic genotype's environment than did the nonplastic genotype in the plastic genotype's environment, and this was associated with the magnitude of phenotypic convergence between native and transplanted genotypes [60].

Knowledge of the ancestor-descendent relationship between populations has provided some of the most striking evidence for PMP. Yeh and Price [61] studied a population of dark-eyed juncos on the campus of the University of California, San Diego, invaders that had left a montane population and successfully colonized this new environment in the 1980s. A six-year study beginning in 1999 found that the warmer coastal environment permitted a longer breeding season, and more offspring per breeding pair, than the montane climate. Despite this increased reproductive output, the population remained stable with little migration. They concluded that, in the absence of plasticity, the population would not have survived, presumably due to higher rates of predation on campus than in the wild [61]. Benthic and limnetic threespine stickleback are relatively nonplastic in the morphological features that distinguish them, but they occupy opposite ends of the morphological reaction norm of ancestral marine stickleback, suggesting that ancestral plasticity was required for these species pairs to occupy their respective niches (the "flexible-stem" hypothesis; [62, 63]; see also [64-75]).

Suggestive evidence for PMP comes from other sources. Phenotypic change is greater in populations experiencing anthropogenic disturbance than in those that are not, and this primarily occurs through plastic responses that may be adaptive [76, 77]. Studies of tolerance limits have shown that many species are not living at the edge of what they can tolerate, suggesting that physiological and molecular plasticity will permit persistence under future climate change [78, 79]. Finally, phylogenetic analyses have allowed intriguing correlational questions to be asked. Pfennig and McGee [43] hypothesized that, if PMP is true, clades with plasticity should be more speciose than sister clades of similar age that lack plasticity, due to a combination of reduced extinction risk, greater opportunities to diversify, and, unrelated to PMP, increased evolvability of plastic traits. Their predictions were supported in three fish and two amphibian lineages. Overall, from climate change to invasive species to phylogenetic approaches, plasticity does seem to be an important contributor to persistence in new or changing environments.

3. Criteria for Demonstrating PMP

Although the above examples demonstrate that plasticity is an important means by which species cope with changing environments, many of these examples only infer PMP without directly demonstrating it. This is in part because the definition of "mediated" in PMP is open to debate. Baldwin envisioned a situation in which nonplastic individuals were less fit than adaptively plastic individuals. If no individuals were plastic, the population as a whole would go extinct, barring (we would add today) evolutionary rescue. This can be called hard PMP and is empirically demonstrated only when plasticity is shown to be responsible for the survival of the individual and/or production of offspring in the new environment. Hard PMP can be contrasted with less ambitious but no less interesting research programs that infer that preexistent plasticity has some adaptive significance in a novel environment, what can be called soft PMP. Soft PMP studies usually cannot rule out alternatives to PMP; for instance, an organism may fortuitously produce an adaptive phenotype in a new environment, but it may have been able to survive and produce offspring even in the absence of this plastic modification. In the absence of evidence for hard PMP, a consilience of evidence from soft PMP studies is required to stack the evidence in favour of PMP. Ideally any study seeking to demonstrate hard PMP will be able to address the following questions.

3.1. Q1: Does Plasticity Preexist Environmental Change? By definition, PMP can only occur if plasticity is retained within the population in the ancestral condition--that is, the potential for plastic modification must preexist the new environment. Many plasticity studies that indirectly infer PMP are not geared to address this question, such as comparisons between plasticity in invasive versus noninvasive species. In such cases the evolution of plasticity after invasion cannot be ruled out--perhaps invasive species have genomic architectures that are more prone to plasticity evolution [80]. In order to demonstrate PMP, there must be some way to measure plasticity immediately upon exposure to a novel environment, or to infer preexistent plasticity from ancestor-derived comparisons or phylogenetic analyses.

Experimental environmental manipulation and studies on natural populations during environmental change have shown that plasticity can indeed preexist environmental change. Some particularly fascinating examples involve the production of novel phenotypes under unnatural environments. Moss grown under zero-gravity conditions developed unusual spiral morphologies [81]; a killer whale at Marine Land learned to catch gulls by baiting them with its fish food, a game it then taught to others [ 82]; the recent northern range expansion of a butterfly species resulted in the production of a novel wing colouration induced by temperature [83]; experiments under unnaturally high carbon dioxide levels altered flowering time and carbon stores in numerous plant species (e.g., [84, 85]); invasive black rat populations developed a novel strategy for accessing pine seeds [86]. Studies on developmental plasticity have shown that tissues can respond in remarkable ways to environmental input, resulting in dramatic morphological changes within an individual. The so-called "two-legged goat effect" was named by West-Eberhard [9] after a goat that was forced to walk on two legs due to a congenital limb defect. This resulted in the development of a "kangaroo-like" skeletal system. Similar observations were made even earlier by Fuld on a "kangaroo-like" two-legged dog [87]. Most recently this effect was observed in the

extant stem tetrapod Polypterus, a fish that can breathe and "walk" on land. When raised in a terrestrial environment, developmental plasticity resulted in morphological changes that improved terrestrial performance and pointed towards a preexistent plasticity explanation for the origins of some aspects of the terrestrial body plan [88].

Comparisons between contemporary ancestral and derived populations may also be used to infer preexistent plasticity. If both forms are similarly plastic, it is tempting to infer that plasticity permitted persistence in the novel environment. However, caution should be exercised: plasticity in the ancestral form must be tested in the derived form's environment [89]. If plasticity is measured in the ancestral form in the ancestral environment, it is possible that, under derived conditions, this plasticity would be hindered, necessitating its reevolution [10]. If the derived form is not plastic but the ancestor is, care should be taken to demonstrate that canalization occurred in the derived form, and not that plasticity subsequently evolved in the contemporary "ancestral" form. This is straightforward to do if multiple derived forms exist that have nonplastic phenotypes representative of opposite ends of the ancestral reaction norm. This is the basis of the "flexible-stem" hypothesis discussed above (e.g., [62]).

3.2. Q2: Is Preexistent Plasticity of Utility in the New Environment? Simply demonstrating preexistent plasticity is not enough, as this plasticity may be maladaptive, adaptive, or neutral in the new environment. Some sort of utility in the new environment needs to be demonstrated. A novel colour phenotype induced by rising temperatures is interesting, but does it have fitness consequences? Others have reviewed the steps taken to demonstrate adaptive plasticity in nature [90], but not all of these steps apply to preexistent plasticity. For instance, associating the degree of plasticity with the extent of environmental heterogeneity will not work if plasticity is retained in ancestral organisms that do not experience the inducing environment. Strong evidence for preexistent plasticity's adaptive significance comes from flexible-stem scenarios (e.g., [62]). Barring ancestor-derived comparisons, selection experiments on individuals that differ in their degree of plasticity, under novel environmental conditions, would be ideal, but studies that logically consider the function of the modified phenotype in the novel environment would suffice. For instance, learned behaviours that exploit novel food resources do not require further justification to show that they have significance within the new environment. No matter the evidence, it is essential that the preexistence of plasticity and the significance of this plasticity in a new environment be demonstrated together; on their own neither is sufficient to demonstrate PMP. Unfortunately, most studies cited as demonstrating PMP stop here [75] and do not actually demonstrate that plasticity itself is essential for persistence.

3.3. Q3: Is Plasticity Responsible for Persistence of the Individual in the New Environment? This is the question that soft PMP studies do not directly demonstrate. Important modeling work and meta-analyses suggest that under some conditions at least individuals would perish without plasticity ([16, 32, 33]; however note that individual and population persistence are generally not separated), but direct empirical demonstrations are limited. For instance, Yeh and Price [61] discovered presumably preexistent plasticity that was of utility in the new environment and inferred its role in persistence by demonstrating that, despite the plastic increase in reproductive output, the population remained relatively stable. They associated this stability, rather than population growth, with increased cat predation and from there concluded that, in the absence of this reproductive plasticity, the population would decline. The logic is sound, but the conclusion is inferred. Empirical studies explicitly linking plasticity with individual persistence are sorely needed.

The nature of the question reveals two different ways of thinking about "mediated" that were not captured by the hard versus soft PMP distinction already made. "Mediated" could be relative to other genotypes in the population. Under competition in a new environment, those individuals with preexistent plasticity may outperform nonplastic individuals, resulting in an increase in the frequency of plastic individuals within the population (e.g., [53]). Nonplastic individuals perish; plastic individuals thrive. However, this tells us little about what would have happened had preexistent plasticity not existed. Would the nonplastic genotypes have fared so poorly in the new environment in the absence of competition? "Mediated" then could also refer to situations in which nonplastic individuals perish in the absence of competition with plastic genotypes. Plasticity does not just give an edge over competitors--it is essential for persistence in the new environment. Baldwin was not explicit in differentiating between these two forms, but current researchers should be. The most obvious way to address this question is to measure survival of plastic and nonplastic phenotypes under novel environmental conditions, together and separately (e.g., [53]).

3.4. Q4: Is Plasticity Responsible for Persistence of the Population in the New Environment? This question is really an extension of the question above. If plasticity is favoured by selection from a pool of individuals that differ in plasticity, or if without plasticity the individual would perish, it stands to reason that such plasticity would also be responsible for the continued survival of the population in the new environment. These questions need to be uncoupled for three reasons. First, individual persistence is an intragenerational process, while population persistence is an intergenerational process. Donnelly et al. [91] conclude a lengthy review on plasticity and rapid adaptation to climate change in trees, insects, and birds, by suggesting that plasticity may have immediate fitness-improving effects, but over longer time periods evolutionary change will be required for populations to persist. This is in line with Baldwin's hypothesis that plasticity could permit survival long enough for necessary genetic changes to occur. This highlights a crucial point in the definition of PMP: for PMP to be satisfied, it does not require that the population survive indefinitely due to plasticity. So long as plasticity can keep enough individuals around for ER to occur, PMP has occurred.

Second, and in association with this, persistence is a sneaky term that researchers may define in different ways. Measures of survival, for instance, will say little about fecundity; but fecundity is required if the population is to persist. Third, individuals and populations survive under different temporal scales and therefore may experience different levels of environmental heterogeneity. Plasticity in some traits may permit individuals to survive in the short term but be insufficient to meet the challenges of longer-term environmental change. Modeling work strongly supports the conclusion that plasticity can be essential for colonization success or population persistence under changing environments ([16, 17]; see evolutionary rescue discussion above), but experimental work in which the long-term colonization and establishment success of plastic and nonplastic genotypes are compared is needed.

3.5. Conclusion. Soft PMP studies tend to infer yes to questions three and four based on empirical affirmations to questions one and two (e.g., [75]). But questions one and two are not sufficient to empirically demonstrate PMP. For instance, surface-dwelling progenitors of cavefish harbour cryptic genetic variation for eye size. Under cave environments, the heat shock protein 90 system is unable to maintain proper function, resulting in the phenotypic expression of this genetic variation. Selection then favours variants in the direction of reduced eye size [92]. Although this study shows that adaptive plasticity leads to the development of smaller eyes in cave-dwelling fish, it cannot rule out the possibility that, in the absence of such plasticity, vestigial eyes would have still evolved. Ideally, hard PMP studies will become more common in the future, by demonstrating that plasticity is preexistent, functionally significant, and directly responsible for the persistence of the individual and the population. Such studies naturally produce tangential questions that are not necessary for demonstrating PMP, but which are interesting in their own right.

4. Additional Questions Raised by PMP

4.1. How Is Preexistent Plasticity Maintained in the Population? Why would plasticity exist before the new environment was encountered? There are numerous possibilities, the first of which has received the least amount of attention. (a) In the case of the colonizing environment having the same environmental fluctuations as the old environment, plasticity may have evolved as an adaptation to the old environment and maintained its adaptive function in the colonizing environment. This tends to be underappreciated because it seems obvious, but often novel environments vary in other factors that may negatively interact with the reaction norm of interest. If a phenotype adaptively responds to temperature in the old environment, the new environment may have the same temperature fluctuations but a higher salinity which inhibits the expression of the temperature-induced reaction norm. Researchers tend to ignore phenotypic similarity between populations, but this is a fruitful avenue for further research, as it could uncover examples of genetic or plastic compensation [10, 93, 94]. (b) Ancestral plasticity may have evolved in the old environment and be sufficient to partially meet the challenges of the new environment, favouring ER [16, 39]. (c) Plasticity may have existed in the ancestral population without being expressed.

Of particular theoretical interest for (c) are instances in which populations live below their tolerance limits. As one example, sea urchins exposed to low pH increased transcript abundance for genes associated with biomineralization. This transcriptional plasticity restored the morphological phenotype, yet was responding to acidity levels never experienced in nature [79]. Physiological stress responses maybe capable of compensating for these rarely-if-ever encountered environments [94]. Unexpressed plasticity may also be caused by the inherent capacity of organismal structure and behaviour to change in response to novel stimuli in a way that permits proper organismal functioning in the novel environment. This is the "two-legged goat effect" described above [9, 88]. Finally, unexpressed plasticity may be caused by cryptic genetic variation (CGV). CGV is defined as standing genetic variation that has no impact on phenotype or fitness under normal conditions, but which is expressed under stressful or novel conditions [95, 96]. CGV can be accumulated due to neutral mutations that occur in unexposed regions of a reaction norm [97], past selection in other environments [98], or the action of phenotypic capacitors like Hsp90 that reduce the phenotypic effects of novel genetic input [92, 99]. Novel conditions express these hidden variants, resulting in increased genetic variance for the trait. The random nature of this revealed plasticity may result in some variants being more fit than others and a subset of CGV being selected. Experimental evidence supports the importance of CGV for PMP and rapid adaptation ([40, 100, 101]; reviewed in [75]). Importantly, the very nature of plasticity enables the accumulation of CGV in two ways [63, 102]. (a) Since plastic phenotypes are only expressed under particular circumstances, if conditions to produce the phenotype are not met, mutations can accumulate in the unexpressed regions of the reaction norm and be revealed in the new environment. (b) The development of plastic phenotypes may require more genes than nonplastic phenotypes. This provides a larger number of nucleotide sites for mutations that have conditional expression [103].

4.2. Under What Circumstances Will PMP Occur? This is perhaps the biggest and most challenging question facing future work on PMP. Genetic compensation occurs when maladaptive plasticity is overcome through evolutionary rescue. That is, there are situations in which a nonplastic genotype is more fit than a plastic genotype, and yet these maladaptively plastic individuals survive, reproduce, and eventually reevolve their old phenotype through a new developmental pathway [10]. Under what situations, then, is adaptive plasticity so essential that without it the individual would perish and the population would go extinct? Just how common is PMP? Is it possible that under most scenarios plasticity is merely beneficial without being necessary for survival?

4.3. What Types of Phenotypes Are Responsible for PMP? It is likely that many of the phenotypes essential for persistence are transitory and/or molecular or physiological and are therefore difficult to measure [104]. Researchers may not even be aware that plasticity is occurring--ancestral and derived populations may be, at a whole-organism level, phenotypically similar, while plasticity occurs behind the scenes [93, 94]. That is, phenotypic similarity may be caused by different developmental networks induced by different environments, and without this plastic compensation, extinction may result. Empirical demonstrations of PMP may become increasingly more common as these molecular phenotypes become more readily accessible.

4.4. Is Variation in Plasticity due to Genetic Variation? Variation in plasticity may not always be due to genetic variation. An individual's degree of plasticity may have to do with the amount of environmental heterogeneity experienced early in life [105]. A consistently expressed environment may cause the expression of a plastic phenotype early in life, followed by an insensitivity to further environmental change, while the experience of random heterogeneity may increase the duration of environmental sensitivity. Thus genetically identical individuals of the same age class may have very different phenotypic responses to a novel environment. Those individuals that persist, and that contribute to the persistence of the population, may persist for reasons other than genetic variation. Furthermore, the role of cross-generational epigenetic inheritance in population persistence is only now being elucidated [75]. For these reasons, unless genotypes are directly measured, one should be cautious about calling individuals with different plasticities different "genotypes"

4.5. Can PMP Be Linked with Ecological Speciation? There are three components of ecological speciation: the colonization of distinct environments; adaptive phenotypic and genetic divergence under divergent selection; and reproductive isolation as a byproduct of divergent selection, with reinforcement as a potential additional step [106]. Plasticity can play an important role in each of these steps of speciation (Table 3), with the most obvious step involving survival and phenotypic divergence in divergent environments. However, preexistent plasticity has the ironic role of prohibiting the next stage of ecological speciation, genetic divergence, by permitting repeated and successful migrations between phenotypically divergent populations [33]. The direct connection between PMP and ecological speciation remains intriguing but an as yet unverified possibility.

5. Conclusion

Over one hundred years ago, Baldwin predicted that plasticity could permit species to survive in changing environments and that plasticity could favour both coincident and correlated variants. He developed these ideas primarily to rescue plasticity from Lamarckian inheritance. In 1953 Simpson [8] concluded that, although intuitive and in line with current evolutionary theory, there was no compelling evidence for the Baldwin effect in nature. Today that story has changed. A consilience of evidence from soft PMP literature is highly suggestive of plasticity's integral role in persistence. Hard PMP can be demonstrated only if preexistent fortuitously beneficial plasticity is measured and directly linked with individual and population persistence. Theoretical and empirical research has proven promising, but much work remains to be done.

Appendices

A. Glossary of Terms

Baldwin Effect. The process whereby plasticity facilitates survival in a new or changed environment and directs the future course of evolution by favouring coincident and correlated variants. Possible outcomes include the evolution of greater plasticity, enhancements to the modified trait, selection on other traits, or genetic assimilation.

Canalization. The developmental stabilization of a phenotype against environmental or genetic perturbations. Canalization could occur for multiple or individual phenotypes along a reaction norm.

Coarse-Grained Environment. A term used to refer to environmental change that occurs between generations, either temporally or, for dispersing organisms, spatially. Contrasted with fine-grained environment.

Coincident Variants. For Baldwin, these were mutations that altered the phenotype in the same direction as the environmental modification, resulting in the eventual loss of plasticity.

Correlated Variants. For Baldwin, these were mutations that arose that improved the performance of other phenotypes that were permitted to survive by plasticity.

Cryptic Genetic Variation. Genetic variation that can accumulate in a population without being phenotypically expressed in normal environments, but which are expressed in novel environments.

Ecological Speciation. The production of new species through divergent selection in divergent environments, producing reproductive isolation as a byproduct.

Evolutionary Rescue. Genetic changes that occur when a population is on the brink of extinction, which improve fitness and prevent extinction.

Flexible Stem. This term is used to describe situations in which two or more, relatively non-plastic, sister species reside in distinct environments and are phenotypically distinct, while the ancestral population is plastic in such a way that at least some of this variation is captured by raising the ancestral population in either derived environment. That is, plasticity in the ancestor led to genetically based phenotypic diversification among daughter species.

Genetic Assimilation. Modern definition: adaptive loss of plasticity. Historical definition: it refers to the evolution of a reaction norm such that a phenotype not produced in the ancestral environment undergoes selection upon expression in a new environment, such that it can now be produced in the ancestral environment. This could involve the loss of plasticity, or shifts in reaction norm height and slope.

Genetic Compensation. The evolutionary rescue of populations experiencing maladaptive plasticity, in a manner that restores the phenotype from its modified state.

Organicists. Name given to an eclectic mix of biologists who argued for an important role for the environment in shaping the phenotype and directing the future course of evolution. This group included James Mark Baldwin, Henry Fairfield Osborn, C. Lloyd Morgan, and Edward Bagnall Poulton, among others.

Organic Selection. Baldwin's term for the survival of plastic individuals and the course of evolution thereby taken.

Orthoplasy. Baldwin's term for the direction of evolution determined by plasticity.

Phenotypic Accommodation. The process whereby genetic and plastic changes to an organism that disrupt its function are compensated by plastic changes in other phenotypes.

Phenotypic Plasticity. The ability of a single genotype to produce multiple environmentally induced phenotypes.

Plastic Compensation. The plastic equivalent of genetic compensation, and a subset of phenotypic accommodation. The plastic rescue of individuals experiencing maladaptive plasticity, in a manner that restores the phenotype from its modified state.

Plasticity-Mediated Persistence. The protection from extinction afforded by plasticity. Divided into hard PMP, where plasticity is essential for survival, and soft PMP, where plasticity is of adaptive significance in the new environment.

Reaction Norm. A function describing all of the phenotypic states possible for a genotype across some environmental variable.

Standing Genetic Variation. Genetic variation for a phenotype that exists in natural populations.

B. Baldwin's Theory of Speciation through Plasticity

Although Baldwin did not have modern theories of speciation to rely on, he did briefly discuss his own theory of speciation involving plasticity. The following theory has been taken largely from chapters 13 and 14 of Baldwin [6].

(a) A population of organisms that have the capacity to learn is geographically split into two identical populations.

(b) Both populations encounter the same novel environment.

(c) There are multiple ways to overcome the challenges imposed by this new environment.

(d) Individuals, through exploration and trial-and-error, discover a successful way to survive.

(e) Through social learning/imitation, this method is passed on to other members of the population and across generations.

(f) By chance, the strategy discovered by one population is different from the strategy discovered by another population.

(g) Coincident variation occurs to assimilate this strategy (making it instinctive to reduce the lag time involved with imitation), while correlated variation improves other aspects of the phenotype associated with this strategy. In this manner the two populations diverge behaviourally, morphologically, physiologically, and genetically.

(h) Although Baldwin had no theory of reproductive isolation, we could round his theory off by concluding that selection on these associated traits produces isolation as a byproduct. Hybrids may be inferior to either form, for instance.

Baldwin's theory is therefore akin to the modern concept of ecological speciation, with the added twist that the populations do not encounter different environments, but rather create their own environments through behavioural plasticity within initially identical environments.

C. Historic versus Modern Definitions of the Baldwin Effect and Genetic Assimilation

Baldwin and Waddington were not always clear writers. They often coined terms that were not accepted by the scientific community. Baldwin's knowledge of inheritance was pre-Mendelian, and the mechanisms by which the Baldwin effect acts are often vague. It is no surprise then that misunderstandings over their ideas have arisen. One such misunderstanding is worth briefly discussing, as the paper it is found in, published in the journal Evolution, has become the definitive statement on the subject. Crispo [107] explicitly differentiates between the Baldwin effect and Waddington's genetic assimilation by attributing to Baldwin the evolution of reaction norm elevation or increased plasticity, and to

Waddington the adaptive loss of plasticity. She makes this error by misreading one particular passage in Baldwin [6] and misinterpreting one experiment of Waddington's. The Baldwin passage, with Crispo's interpretation in brackets, is as follows:

   many functions may be passably performed
   through accommodation, supplementing congenital
   (heritable) characters, which would be better
   performed were the congenital characters
   strengthened (i.e., if further phenotypic change
   occurred). Congenital variation would in these
   cases by seizing upon this additional utility (plasticity),
   carry evolution on farther than it had
   gone before (i.e., result in heritable changes in
   form)... this would give the gradual shifting of
   the congenital mean toward the full endowment
   (phenotypic optimum) ([6], pp. 209-210).


The problem arises from Crispo's interpretation of this passage: "An empirical example of shifts in mean phenotypic value without a change in level of plasticity is documented..." Crispo seems to interpret Baldwin as stating that plasticity of a trait can remain unchanged while the trait mean in either environment evolves in parallel, producing a change in reaction norm height but not slope (although in other places she includes an increase in slope to be another aspect of the Baldwin effect). Had she fully quoted Baldwin's statement, Baldwin's meaning would have become clear. As Baldwin writes:

   Congenital variation would in these cases by
   seizing upon this additional utility carry evolution
   farther than it had gone before. For example,
   muscular strength in biting would in no way
   prevent the evolution of hardness of teeth. The
   accommodation factor (muscular strength) would
   be gradually dispensed with, since the most unsuccessful
   of those which depended upon accommodation
   would be eliminated. (brackets mine)


In other words, Baldwin is not talking about trait means versus plasticity, as Crispo wants him to, but is rather talking about the evolution of nonplastic, correlated traits (hardness of teeth) and the loss of plasticity (muscular strength). In this example, Baldwin is suggesting that plasticity in trait A enables population persistence long enough for nonplastic trait B to evolve a function that supplants trait A. Plasticity in trait A had been costly due to the time required for trait A to respond to the environment; the result was its replacement by trait B and, potentially, the loss of the now-unnecessary plasticity of trait A. Crispo's interpretation causes her to define the Baldwin effect as follows:

   In the Baldwin effect, selection may act to change
   mean trait values without changing the level
   of plasticity in the population, or, alternately,
   selection acting on the phenotype can result in
   increases in the level of plasticity (because the most
   plastic individuals possess the most extreme phenotype
   and are thus positively selected) ... Genetic
   assimilation, conversely, should act to decrease
   plasticity (i.e, increase canalization) in a population
   within a given range of environmental conditions
   if an increase in canalization is adaptive.


On my reading this is an incorrect definition of the historical use of this term. The Baldwin effect was always interpreted to include the loss of plasticity as one form of orthoplasy [8]. Genetic assimilation similarly does not conform to Crispo's definition.

Crispo quotes Waddington's definition of genetic assimilation as a process "by which a phenotypic character, which initially is produced only in response to some environmental influence, becomes, through a process of selection, taken over by the genotype, so that it is formed even in the absence of the environmental influence which had at first been necessary ([108] quoted in 107])" She then interprets this to mean that "the environment induces phenotypes that are adaptive, and then selection on the developmental system acts to reduce responsiveness to the environment (i.e., reduced plasticity), so that the induced phenotype becomes "inherited" (i.e., canalized) after a number of generations of exposure to the environmental stimulus [107]" This interpretation, in which a novel phenotype is plastically produced and then "assimilated" into the genotype as an adaptively nonplastic (or less plastic) trait, is the current prevailing definition of genetic assimilation, but it is not Waddington's definition. First, Waddington defined canalization, not as the loss of plasticity, but as the buffering of the phenotype from environmental or genetic perturbations, and this could importantly include plastic phenotypes [9, 109, 110]. If a reaction norm that responded to temperature maintained its shape against novel mutations or nontemperature environmental factors, that plastic reaction norm would be canalized under Waddington's definition. If one phenotype along a reaction norm resisted environmental or genetic perturbations, but a phenotype at the other end of the reaction norm revealed increased phenotypic variation under the same perturbations, the first phenotype but not the second would be canalized under Waddington's definition. Thus canalization is broader than genetic assimilation, and plasticity itself can be canalized. Second, Waddington's definition of genetic assimilation nowhere requires the loss of plasticity. So long as a phenotype that could not be produced in the old environment undergoes selection in the new environment such that it is now produced in the old environment, genetic assimilation has occurred. This is most readily understood through the loss of plasticity (which is a form of increased canalization) but could just as easily occur with a shift in the height or increase in the slope of plasticity. This is exactly how Waddington defines it in his 1959 paper [111] which Crispo believed Waddington to have misinterpreted. Here Waddington imposed selection on fruit flies for a phenotype induced by high salt concentration (large number of anal papillae) that was also associated with survival. After several generations a large number of anal papillae could now develop under low salt concentrations. This is genetic assimilation. It also happens that this selection resulted in increased plasticity of the phenotype--even more anal papillae were induced by high salt concentrations in these selected lines--so there were both an increase in the height (resulting in genetic assimilation) and an increase in the slope of the reaction norm. This is completely consistent with Waddington's defintion of genetic assimilation and shows that he did not take canalization and genetic assimilation to be equivalent.

Ultimately, terms will be defined based on modern usage and their usefulness in describing natural phenomena. The Baldwin effect, genetic assimilation, and canalization have been defined in numerous review papers following Crispo's definition, including some of my own (e.g., [93])--but it is at least worth noting that this is removed from how the generators of these terms originally used them, and too narrow of definitions may prevent the generation of new hypotheses. For instance, the idea that plasticity can be selected to be canalized against other environmental factors has received little attention.

D. Learning and Invasive Success

Although Baldwin [6] allowed for all forms of plasticity (morphological, physiological, behavioural), learning was of especial interest to him. Learning has been called "hypervariable" plasticity [112] because it can result in the production of numerous behavioural (and in conjunction morphological, physiological) phenotypes in response to a diverse array of novel conditions. A particularly rich literature has developed around the importance of learning in the successful colonization of new habitats. Of especial interest are the dual roles of innovation through exploratory behaviour, and social learning through copying [113]. Organisms that exhibit exploratory behaviour are more likely to hit upon novel adaptive behaviours for surviving in novel environments; organisms that have strong social learning are more likely to mimic and retain adaptive behaviours. A combination of the two can permit innovation while reducing the risks of unnecessary exploration, what is known as the adaptive flexibility hypothesis [113]. For instance, invading sparrows showed greater innovation through their willingness to approach and consume novel foods than did long-established noninvasive sparrows of the same species [114]. Learning enables colonization of urban environments [77] and tends to be higher in invasive species than the conspecific species they displace [115-117]. Invasive species tend to show more innovative behaviour in their home range than noninvasive species [118,119]. Brain size can act as a proxy for learning capacity, and has been shown to be positively associated with survival and invasive success [120-122], increased innovation [123], increased tolerance of environmental uncertainty [124], and reduced mortality rates [125]. For instance, a comparison of 446 introduction attempts in mammals found that successful colonization was significantly associated with brain size, even after accounting for life-history, body size, mating strategy, habitat generalism, and the number of released individuals [121]. Experimental evidence also supports PMP. Blue tits synchronize their breeding time with peak food availability by learning from the previous year's environmental cues. Birds fed by researchers while they were nestling laid eggs later the following year, missing the peak production of their natural food. Control birds that relied on natural food showed no such delay the following year [126]. Thus plasticity to the wrong cues can be maladaptive, but a lack of responsiveness could be similarly maladaptive [113,127]. Over one hundred years after Baldwin, evidence is rapidly accumulating that support his initial hypothesis that learning in particular is an important contributor to PMP.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/416497

Conflict of Interests

The author declares that there is no conflict of interests regarding the publication of this paper.

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to thank the insights of S Rogers, L Harder, G Chua, J Vamosi, C Rollian, and an anonymous reviewer. The author held a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) Vanier Canada Graduate Scholarship during the writing of this paper.

References

[1] D. Henry, "Aristotle on the mechanism of inheritance," Journal of the History of Biology, vol. 39, no. 3, pp. 425-455, 2006.

[2] J. B. P. A. Lamarck, "On the influence of circumstances on the actions and habits of animals, and that of the actions and habits of living bodies, as causes which modify their organization," The American Naturalist, vol. 22, no. 264, pp. 1054-1066, 1888.

[3] C. Darwin, The Origin of Species, John Murray, London, UK, 1859.

[4] B. Charlesworth and D. Charlesworth, "Darwin and genetics," Genetics, vol. 183, no. 3, pp. 757-766, 2009.

[5] W. Johannsen, "The genotype conception of heredity," The American Naturalist, vol. 45, pp. 129-159, 1911.

[6] J. M. Baldwin, Development and Evolution, Macmillan, New York, NY, USA, 1902.

[7] G. J. Romanes, Darwin, and After Darwin. II. Post-Darwinian Questions, Heredity and Utility, Open Court Publishing Company, Chicago, Ill, USA, 1895.

[8] G. G. Simpson, "The Baldwin effect," Evolution, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 110-117, 1953.

[9] M. J. West-Eberhard, Developmental Plasticity and Evolution, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2003.

[10] G. F. Grether, "Environmental change, phenotypic plasticity, and genetic compensation," The American Naturalist, vol. 166, no. 4, pp. E115-E123, 2005.

[11] L.-M. Chevin, R. Gallet, R. Gomulkiewicz, R. D. Holt, and S. Fellous, "Phenotypic plasticity in evolutionary rescue experiments," Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological sciences, vol. 368, no. 1610, 2013.

[12] R. D. H. Barrett and A. P. Hendry, "Evolutionary rescue under environmental change?" in Behavioural Responses to a Changing World, U. Candolin and B. B. M. Wong, Eds., Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2012.

[13] S. A. B. Roels and J. K. Kelly, "Rapid evolution caused by pollinator loss in Mimulus guttatus," Evolution, vol. 65, no. 9, pp. 2541-2552, 2011.

[14] G. Bell and A. Gonzalez, "Adaptation and evolutionary rescue in metapopulations experiencing environmental deterioration," Science, vol. 332, no. 6035, pp. 1327-1330, 2011.

[15] H. A. Lindsey, J. Gallie, S. Taylor, and B. Kerr, "Evolutionary rescue from extinction is contingent on a lower rate of environmental change," Nature, vol. 494, no. 7438, pp. 463-467, 2013.

[16] L.-M. Chevin and R. Lande, "When do adaptive plasticity and genetic evolution prevent extinction of a density-regulated population?" Evolution, vol. 64, no. 4, pp. 1143-1150, 2010.

[17] O. Vedder, S. Bouwhuis, and B. C. Sheldon, "Quantitative assessment of the importance of phenotypic plasticity in adaptation to climate change in wild bird populations," PLoS Biology, vol. 11, no. 7, Article ID e1001605, 2013.

[18] A. P. Moller, D. Rubolini, and E. Lehikoinen, "Populations of migratory bird species that did not show a phenological response to climate change are declining," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 105, no. 42, pp. 16195-16200, 2008.

[19] N. Saino, D. Rubolini, E. Lehikoinen et al., "Climate change effects on migration phenology may mismatch brood parasitic cuckoos and their hosts," Biology Letters, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 539-541, 2009.

[20] E. Post and M. C. Forchhammer, "Climate change reduces reproductive success of an Arctic herbivore through trophic mismatch," Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, vol. 363, no. 1501, pp. 2369-2375, 2008.

[21] A. Charmantier, R. H. McCleery, L. R. Cole, C. Perrins, L. E. B. Kruuk, and B. C. Sheldon, "Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in response to climate change in a wild bird population," Science, vol. 320, no. 5877, pp. 800-803, 2008.

[22] N. T. Jones, B. C. Husband, and A. S. MacDougall, "Reproductive system of a mixed-mating plant responds to climate perturbation by increased selfing," Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 280, no. 1766, Article ID 20131336, 2013.

[23] M. E. Visser, L. te Marvelde, and M. E. Lof, "Adaptive phenological mismatches of birds and their food in a warming world," Journal of Ornithology, vol. 153, no. 1, pp. 75-84, 2012.

[24] T. E. Reed, V. Grtoan, S. Jenouvrier, B.-E. Steher, and M. E. Visser, "Population growth in a wild bird is buffered against phenological mismatch," Science, vol. 340, no. 6131, pp. 488-491, 2013.

[25] M. van Asch, L. Salis, L. J. M. Holleman, B. van Lith, and M. E. Visser, "Evolutionary response of the egg hatching date of a herbivorous insect under climate change," Nature Climate Change, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 244-248, 2013.

[26] T. D. Price and D. Sol, "Introduction: genetics of colonizing species," The American Naturalist, vol. 172, no. 1, pp. S1-S3, 2008.

[27] S. Y. Strauss, C. O. Webb, and N. Salamin, "Exotic taxa less related to native species are more invasive," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 103, no. 15, pp. 5841-5845, 2006.

[28] V. Matzek, "Trait values, not trait plasticity, best explain invasive species' performance in a changing environment," PLoS ONE, vol. 7, no. 10, Article ID e48821, 2012.

[29] P. Dostal, W. Dawson, M. van Kleunen, L. H. Keser, and M. Fischer, "Central European plant species from more productive habitats are more invasive at a global scale," Global Ecology and Biogeography, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 64-72, 2013.

[30] S. M. Scheiner, "The genetics of phenotypic plasticity: XII: temporal and spatial heterogeneity," Ecology and Evolution, vol. 3, no. 13, pp. 4596-4609, 2013.

[31] K. Schiffers, E. C. Bourne, S. Lavergne, W. Thuiller, and J. M. J. Travis, "Limited evolutionary rescue of locally adapted populations facing climate change," Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological sciences, vol. 368, no. 1610, 2013.

[32] J. Hollander, "Testing the grain-size model for the evolution of phenotypic plasticity," Evolution, vol. 62, no. 6, pp. 1381-1389, 2008.

[33] X. Thibert-Plante and A. P. Hendry, "The consequences of phenotypic plasticity for ecological speciation," Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 326-342, 2011.

[34] D. Bonte, B. Bossuyt, and L. Lens, "Aerial dispersal plasticity under different wind velocities in a salt marsh wolf spider," Behavioral Ecology, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 438-443, 2007

[35] E. Imbert and O. Ronce, "Phenotypic plasticity for dispersal ability in the seed heteromorphic Crepis sancta (Asteraceae)," Oikos, vol. 93, no. 1, pp. 126-134, 2001.

[36] C. A. Pearl, M. J. Adams, G. S. Schuytema, and A. V Nebeker, "Behavioral responses of anuran larvae to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United States," Journal of Herpetology, vol. 37, no. 3, pp. 572-576, 2003.

[37] C. L. Richards, R. L. Walls, J. P Bailey, R. Parameswaran, T. George, and M. Pigliucci, "Plasticity in salt tolerance traits allows for invasion of novel habitat by Japanese knotweed s. l. (Fallopian japonica and F. xbohemica, polygonaceae)," American Journal of Botany, vol. 95, no. 8, pp. 931-942, 2008.

[38] J. Grangier and P J. Lester, "Behavioral plasticity mediates asymmetric competition between invasive wasps and native ants," Communicative and Integrative Biology, vol. 5, no. 2, pp. 127-129, 2012.

[39] R. Lande, "Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation," Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 22, no. 7, pp. 1435-1446, 2009.

[40] C. E. Lee, M. Kiergaard, G. W. Gelembiuk, B. D. Eads, and M. Posavi, "Pumping ions: Rapid parallel evolution of ionic regulation following habitat invasions," Evolution, vol. 65, no. 8, pp. 2229-2244, 2011.

[41] C. L. Richards, O. Bossdorf, N. Z. Muth, J. Gurevitch, and M. Pigliucci, "Jack of all trades, master of some? On the role of phenotypic plasticity in plant invasions," Ecology Letters, vol. 9, no. 8, pp. 981-993, 2006.

[42] C. L. Pohlman, A. B. Nicotra, and B. R. Murray, "Geographic range size, seedling ecophysiology and phenotypic plasticity in Australian Acacia species," Journal of Biogeography, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 341-351, 2005.

[43] D. W. Pfennig and M. McGee, "Resource polyphenism increases species richness: a test of the hypothesis," Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 365, no. 1540, pp. 577-591, 2010.

[44] W. Dawson, R. P Rohr, M. van Kleunen, and M. Fischer, "Alien plant species with a wider global distribution are better able to capitalize on increased resource availability," New Phytologist, vol. 194, no. 3, pp. 859-867, 2012.

[45] L. Bagny Beilhe, H. Delatte, S. A. Juliano, D. Fontenille, and S. Quilici, "Ecological interactions in Aedes species on Reunion Island," Medical and Veterinary Entomology, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 387-397, 2013.

[46] O. Bossdorf, A. Lipowsky, and D. Prati, "Selection of readapted populations allowed Senecio inaequidens to invade central Europe," Diversity and Distributions, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 676-685, 2008.

[47] S. Matesanz, E. Gianoli, and F. Valladares, "Global change and the evolution of phenotypic plasticity in plants," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1206, pp. 35-55, 2010.

[48] S. E. Sultan, T. Horgan-Kobelski, L. M. Nichols, C. E. Riggs, and R. K. Waples, "A resurrection study reveals rapid adaptive evolution within populations of an invasive plant," Evolutionary Applications, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 266-278, 2013.

[49] C. C. Daehler, "Performance comparisons of co-occurring native and invasive plants: implications for conservation research," Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, vol. 34, pp. 183-211, 2003.

[50] J. L. Funk, "Differences in plasticity between invasive and native plants from a low resource environment," Journal of Ecology, vol. 96, no. 6, pp. 1162-1173, 2008.

[51] D. Bachmann, S. Both, H. Bruelheide et al., "Functional trait similarity of native and invasive herb species in subtropical China-Environment-specific differences are the key," Environmental and Experimental Botany, vol. 83, pp. 82-92, 2012.

[52] P E. Hulme, "Phenotypic plasticity and plant invasions: is it all Jack?" Functional Ecology, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 3-7, 2008.

[53] K. Engel and R. Tollrian, "Inducible defences as key adaptations for the successful invasion of Daphnia lumholtzi in North America?" Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 276, no. 1663, pp. 1865-1873, 2009.

[54] A. M. Davidson, M. Jennions, and A. B. Nicotra, "Do invasive species show higher phenotypic plasticity than native species and, if so, is it adaptive? A meta-analysis," Ecology Letters, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 419-431, 2011.

[55] M. A. Hahn, M. van Kleunen, and H. Muller-Scharer, "Increased phenotypic plasticity to climate may have boosted the invasion success of polyploid Centaurea stoebe," PLoS ONE, vol. 7, no. 11, Article ID e50284, 2012.

[56] G. C. Trussell and L. D. Smith, "Induced defenses in response to an invading crab predator: an explanation of historical and geographic phenotypic change," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 97, no. 5, pp. 2123-2127, 2000.

[57] R. D. Moore, R. A. Griffiths, C. M. O'Brien, A. Murphy, and D. Jay, "Induced defences in an endangered amphibian in response to an introduced snake predator," Oecologia, vol. 141, no. 1, pp. 139-147, 2004.

[58] S. D. Peacor, S. Allesina, R. L. Riolo, and M. Pascual, "Phenotypic plasticity opposes species invasions by altering fitness surface," PLoS Biology, vol. 4, no. 11, p. e372, 2006.

[59] R. B. Langerhans and T. J. DeWitt, "Plasticity constrained: overgeneralized induction cues cause maladaptive phenotypes," Evolutionary Ecology Research, vol. 4, no. 6, pp. 857-870, 2002.

[60] K. Donohue, E. H. Pyle, D. Messiqua, M. Shane Heschel, and J. Schmitt, "Adaptive divergence in plasticity in natural populations of Impatiens capensis and its consequences for performance in novel habitats," Evolution,vol. 55, no. 4, pp. 692-702, 2001.

[61] P J. Yeh and T. D. Price, "Adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the successful colonization of a novel environment," The American Naturalist, vol. 164, no. 4, pp. 531-542, 2004.

[62] M. A. Wund, J. A. Baker, B. Clancy, J. L. Golub, and S. A. Foster, "A test of the "flexible stem" model of evolution: ancestral plasticity, genetic accommodation, and morphological divergence in the threespine stickleback radiation," American Naturalist, vol. 172, no. 4, pp. 449-462, 2008.

[63] M. A. Wund, S. Valena, S. Wood, and J. A. Baker, "Ancestral-plasticity and allometry in threespine stickleback reveal phenotypes associated with derived, freshwater ecotypes," Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, vol. 105, no. 3, pp. 573-583, 2012.

[64] J. R. Stauffer Jr. and E. van Snik Gray, "Phenotypic plasticity: its role in trophic radiation and explosive speciation in cichlids (Teleostei: Cichlidae)," Animal Biology, vol. 54, no. 2, pp. 137-158, 2004.

[65] J. J. Kolbe and J. B. Losos, "Hind-limb length plasticity in Anolis carolinensis," Journal of Herpetology, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. 674-678, 2005.

[66] I. Gomez-Mestre and D. R. Buchholz, "Developmental plasticity mirrors differences among taxa in spadefoot toads linking plasticity and diversity," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 103, no. 50, pp. 19021-19026, 2006.

[67] J. Masel, O. D. King, and H. Maughan, "The loss of adaptive plasticity during long periods of environmental stasis," The American Naturalist, vol. 169, no. 1, pp. 38-46, 2007

[68] K. A. Shaw, M. L. Scotti, and S. A. Foster, "Ancestral plasticity and the evolutionary diversification of courtship behaviour in threespine sticklebacks," Animal Behaviour, vol. 73, no. 3, pp. 415-422, 2007.

[69] F. Aubret and R. Shine, "Genetic assimilation and the postcolonization erosion of phenotypic plasticity in island tiger snakes," Current Biology, vol. 19, no. 22, pp. 1932-1936, 2009.

[70] F. Aubret and R. Shine, "Fitness costs may explain the postcolonisation erosion of phenotypic plasticity," The Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 213, no. 5, pp. 735-739, 2010.

[71] R. J. S. McCairns and L. Bernatchez, "Adaptive divergence between freshwater and marine sticklebacks: insights into the role of phenotypic plasticity from an integrated analysis of candidate gene expression," Evolution, vol. 64, no. 4, pp. 1029-1047, 2010.

[72] T. Schwander and O. Leimar, "Genes as leaders and followers in evolution," Trends in Ecology and Evolution, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 143-151, 2011.

[73] R. Rajakumar, D. San Mauro, M. B. Dijkstra et al., "Ancestral developmental potential facilitates parallel evolution in ants," Science, vol. 335, no. 6064, pp. 79-82, 2012.

[74] P K. Diggle and J. S. Miller, "Developmental plasticity, genetic assimilation, and the evolutionary diversification of sexual expression in Solanum," The American Journal of Botany, vol. 100, no. 6, pp. 1050-1060, 2013.

[75] C. D. Schlichting and M. A. Wund, "Phenotypic plasticity and epigenetic marking: an assessment of evidence for genetic accommodation," Evolution, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 656-672, 2014.

[76] E. Crispo, J. D. DiBattista, C. Correa et al., "The evolution of phenotypic plasticity in response to anthropogenic disturbance," Evolutionary Ecology Research, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 47-66, 2010.

[77] D. Sol, O. Lapiedra, and C. Gonzalez-Lagos, "Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city," Animal Behaviour, vol. 85, no. 5, pp. 1101-1112, 2013.

[78] G. N. Somero, "The physiology of climate change: how potentials for acclimatization and genetic adaptation will determine "winners" and 'losers'," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 213, no. 6, pp. 912-920, 2010.

[79] S. Martin, S. Richier, M.-L. Pedrotti et al., "Early development and molecular plasticity in the Mediterranean sea urchin Paracentrotus lividus exposed to CO2-driven acidification," The Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 214, no. 8, pp. 1357-1368, 2011.

[80] A. R. Leichty, D. W. Pfennig, C. D. Jones, and K. S. Pfennig, "Relaxed genetic constraint is ancestral to the evolution of phenotypic plasticity," Integrative and Comparative Biology, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 16-30, 2012.

[81] V. D. Kern, J. M. Schwuchow, D. W. Reed et al., "Gravitropic moss cells default to spiral growth on the clinostat and in microgravity during space flight," Planta, vol. 221, no. 1, pp. 149-157, 2005.

[82] S. Szerdy, "Clever whale uses fish to catch seagulls," USA Today, 2005.

[83] J. M. Otaki, A. Hiyama, M. Iwata, and T. Kudo, "Phenotypic plasticity in the range-margin population of the lycaenid butterfly Zizeeria maha," BMC Evolutionary Biology, vol. 10, article 252, 2010.

[84] J. Y. C. Reekie, P R. Hicklenton, and E. G. Reekie, "Effects of elevated CO2 on time of flowering in four short-day and four long-day species," Canadian Journal of Botany, vol. 72, no. 4, pp. 533-538, 1994.

[85] E. G. Reekie, G. MacDougall, I. Wong, and P R. Hicklenton, "Effect of sink size on growth response to elevated atmospheric CO2 within the genus brassica," Canadian Journal of Botany, vol. 76, no. 5, pp. 829-835, 1998.

[86] R. Aisner and J. Terkel, "Ontogeny of pine cone opening behaviour in the black rat, Rattus rattus," Animal Behaviour, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 327-336, 1992.

[87] J. Huxley, Essays in Popular Science, Reprint of 1926 Edition, Penguin, London, UK, 1937

[88] E. M. Standen, T. Y. Du, and H. C. E. Larsson, "Developmental plasticity and the origin of tetrapods," Nature, vol. 513, pp. 54-58, 2014.

[89] M. R. J. Morris, R. Richard, E. H. Leder, R. D. H. Barrett, N. Aubin-Horth, and S. M. Rogers, "Gene expression plasticity evolves in response to colonization of freshwater lakes in threespine stickleback," Molecular Ecology, vol. 23, pp. 3226-3240, 2014.

[90] P. Doughty and D. Reznick, "Patterns and analysis of adaptive phenotypic plasticity in animals," in Phenotypic Plasticity: Functional and Conceptual, T. J. DeWitt and S. M. Scheiner, Eds., Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2004.

[91] A. Donnelly, A. Caffarra, C. T. Kelleher et al., "Surviving in a warmer world: environmental and genetic responses," Climate Research, vol. 53, no. 3, pp. 245-262, 2012.

[92] N. Rohner, D. F. Jarosz, J. E. Kowalko et al., "Cryptic variation in morphological evolution: HSP90 as a capacitor for loss of eyes in cavefish," Science, vol. 342, no. 6164, pp. 1372-1375, 2013.

[93] M. R. J. Morris and S. M. Rogers, "Overcoming maladaptive plasticity through plastic compensation," Current Zoology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 526-536, 2013.

[94] P M. Schulte, "What is environmental stress? Insights from fish living in a variable environment," The Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 217, no. 1, pp. 23-34, 2014.

[95] G. Gibson and I. Dworkin, "Uncovering cryptic genetic variation," Nature Reviews Genetics, vol. 5, no. 9, pp. 681-690, 2004.

[96] C. D. Schlichting, "Hidden reaction norms, cryptic genetic variation, and evolvability," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1133, pp. 187-203, 2008.

[97] C. K. Ghalambor, J. K. McKay, S. P. Carroll, and D. N. Reznick, "Adaptive versus non-adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the potential for contemporary adaptation in new environments," Functional Ecology, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 394-407, 2007

[98] D. Schluter and G. L. Conte, "Genetics and ecological speciation.," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 106, pp. 9955-9962, 2009.

[99] S. F. Levy and M. L. Siegal, "Network hubs buffer environmental variation in Saccharomyces cerevisiae," PLoS Biology, vol. 6, no. 11, article e264, pp. 2588-2604, 2008.

[100] E. J. Hayden, E. Ferrada, and A. Wagner, "Cryptic genetic variation promotes rapid evolutionary adaptation in an RNA enzyme," Nature, vol. 474, no. 7349, pp. 92-95, 2011.

[101] K. Mcguigan, N. Nishimura, M. Currey, D. Hurwit, and W. A. Cresko, "Cryptic genetic variation and body size evolution in threespine stickleback," Evolution, vol. 65, no. 4, pp. 1203-1211, 2011.

[102] A. P. Moczek, S. Sultan, S. Foster et al., "The role of developmental plasticity in evolutionary innovation," Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 278, no. 1719, pp. 2705-2713, 2011.

[103] D. W. Pfennig, M. A. Wund, E. C. Snell-Rood, T. Cruickshank, C. D. Schlichting, and A. P. Moczek, "Phenotypic plasticity's impacts on diversification and speciation," Trends in Ecology and Evolution, vol. 25, no. 8, pp. 459-467, 2010.

[104] N. Aubin-Horth and S. C. P. Renn, "Genomic reaction norms: using integrative biology to understand molecular mechanisms of phenotypic plasticity," Molecular Ecology, vol. 18, no. 18, pp. 3763-3780, 2009.

[105] W. E. Frankenhuis and K. Panchanathan, "Individual differences in developmental plasticity may result from stochastic sampling," Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 336-347, 2011.

[106] D. Schluter, "Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative," Science, vol. 323, no. 5915, pp. 737-741, 2009.

[107] E. Crispo, "The Baldwin effect and genetic assimilation: Revisiting two mechanisms of evolutionary change mediated by phenotypic plasticity," Evolution, vol. 61, no. 11, pp. 2469-2479, 2007.

[108] C. H. Waddington, "Genetic Assimilation," Advances in Genetics, vol. 10, pp. 257-293, 1961.

[109] C. H. Waddington, "Genetic assimilation of an acquired character," Evolution, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 118-126, 1953.

[110] M. L. Siegal and A. Bergman, "Waddington's canalization revisited: developmental stability and evolution," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 99, no. 16, pp. 10528-10532, 2002.

[111] C. H. Waddington, "Canalization of development and genetic assimilation of acquired characters," Nature, vol. 183, no. 4676, pp. 1654-1655, 1959.

[112] E. C. Snell-Rood, J. D. van Dyken, T. Cruickshank, M. J. Wade, and A. P. Moczek, "Toward a population genetic framework of developmental evolution: the costs, limits, and consequences of phenotypic plasticity," BioEssays, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 71-81, 2010.

[113] T. F. Wright, J. R. Eberhard, E. A. Hobson, M. L. Avery, and M. A. Russello, "Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis," Ethology Ecology and Evolution, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 393-404, 2010.

[114] L. B. Martin II and L. Fitzgerald, "A taste for novelty in invading house sparrows, Passer domesticus," Behavioral Ecology, vol. 16, no. 4, pp. 702-707, 2005.

[115] B. A. Hazlett, P. Acquistapace, and F. Gherardi, "Differences in memory capabilities in invasive and native crayfish," Journal of Crustacean Biology, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 439-448, 2002.

[116] R. J. Roudez, T. Glover, and J. S. Weis, "Learning in an invasive and a native predatory crab," Biological Invasions, vol. 10, no. 8, pp. 1191-1196, 2008.

[117] M. Sutter and T. J. Kawecki, "Influence of learning on range expansion and adaptation to novel habitats," Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 22, no. 11, pp. 2201-2214, 2009.

[118] D. Sol and L. Lefebvre, "Behavioural flexibility predicts invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand," Oikos, vol. 90, no. 3, pp. 599-605, 2000.

[119] D. Sol, S. Timmermans, and L. Lefebvre, "Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds," Animal Behaviour, vol. 63, no. 3, pp. 495-502, 2002.

[120] D. Sol, R. P. Duncan, T. M. Blackburn, P. Cassey, and L. Lefebvre, "Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102, no. 15, pp. 5460-5465, 2005.

[121] D. Sol, S. Bacher, S. M. Reader, and L. Lefebvre, "Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments," American Naturalist, vol. 172, no. 1, pp. S63-S71, 2008.

[122] M. Vall-Llosera and D. Sol, "A global risk assessment for the success of bird introductions," Journal of Applied Ecology, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 787-795, 2009.

[123] S. M. Reader and K. N. Laland, "Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 99, no. 7, pp. 4436-4441, 2002.

[124] C. Schuck-Paim, W. J. Alonso, and E. B. Ottoni, "Cognition in an ever-changing world: climatic variability is associated with brain size

in neotropical parrots," Brain, Behavior and Evolution, vol. 71, no. 3, pp. 200-215, 2008.

[125] D. Sol, T. Szekely, A. Liker, and L. Lefebvre, "Big-brained birds survive better in nature," Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 274, no. 1611, pp. 763-769, 2007

[126] F. Grieco, A. J. van Noordwijk, and M. E. Visser, "Evidence for the effect of learning on timing of reproduction in blue tits," Science, vol. 296, no. 5565, pp. 136-138, 2002.

[127] R. Dukas, "Effects of learning on evolution: robustness, innovation and speciation," Animal Behaviour, vol. 85, no. 5, pp. 1023-1030, 2013.

[128] J. K. Pritchard and A. di Rienzo, "Adaptation: not by sweeps alone," Nature Reviews Genetics, vol. 11, no. 10, pp. 665-667, 2010.

[129] H. A. Orr, "The population genetics of adaptation: the distribution of factors fixed during adaptive evolution," Evolution, vol. 52, no. 4, pp. 935-949, 1998.

[130] B. Charlesworth and D. Charlesworth, Elements of Evolutionary Genetics, Roberts and Company Publishers, Greenwood Village, Colo, USA, 2010.

[131] M. C. S. Soares, M. Lurling, and V. L. M. Huszar, "Growth and temperature-related phenotypic plasticity in the cyanobacterium Cylindrospermopsis raciborskii," Phycological Research, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 61-67, 2013.

[132] H. J. Dalmagro, F. A. de Lobo, G. L. Vourlitis et al., "Photosynthetic parameters of two invasive tree species of the Brazilian Pantanal in response to seasonal flooding," Photosynthetica, vol. 51, no. 2, pp. 281-294, 2013.

[133] A. Monty, J.-P. Bizoux, J. Escarre, and G. Mahy, "Rapid plant invasion in distinct climates involves different sources of phenotypic variation," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 1, Article ID e55627, 2013.

[134] Z. Munzbergova, V. Hadincova, J. Wild, and J. Kindlmannova, "Variability in the contribution of different life stages to population growth as a key factor in the invasion success of Pinus strobus," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 2, Article ID e56953, 2013.

[135] A. T. Pahl, J. Kollmann, A. Mayer, and S. Haider, "No evidence for local adaptation in an invasive alien plant: field and greenhouse experiments tracing a colonization sequence" Annals of Botany, vol. 112, no. 9, pp. 1921-1930, 2013.

[136] L. Thouvenot, J. Haury, and G. Thiebaut, "Seasonal plasticity of Ludwigia grandiflora under light and water depth gradients: an outdoor mesocosm experiment," Flora, vol. 208, no. 7, pp. 430-437, 2013.

[137] I. S. Travlos, "Responses of invasive silverleaf nightshade (Solanum elaeagnifolium) populations to varying soil water availability," Phytoparasitica, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 41-48, 2013.

[138] G. L. Vourlitis and J. L. Kroon, "Growth and resource use of the invasive grass, pampasgrass (Cortaderia selloana), in response to nitrogen and water availability," Weed Science, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 117-125, 2013.

[139] H.-J. T. Hoving, W. F. Gilly, U. Markaida et al., "Extreme plasticity in life-history strategy allows a migratory predator (jumbo squid) to cope with a changing climate," Global Change Biology, vol. 19, no. 7, pp. 2089-2103, 2013.

[140] E. J. Kistner and M. F. Dybdahl, "Adaptive responses and invasion: the role of plasticity and evolution in snail shell morphology," Ecology and Evolution, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 424-436, 2013.

[141] M. Buric, A. Kouba, and P. Kozak, "Reproductive plasticity in freshwater invaders: from long-term sperm storage to parthenogenesis," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 10, Article ID e77597, 2013.

[142] A. Ferrer, S. Dorn, and D. Mazzi, "Cross-generational effects of temperature on flight performance, and associated life-history traits in an insect," Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 26, no. 11, pp. 2321-2330, 2013.

[143] K. Hidalgo, M. Laparie, R. Bical et al., "Metabolic fingerprinting of the responses to salinity in the invasive ground beetle Merizodus soledadinus at the Kerguelen Islands," Journal of Insect Physiology, vol. 59, no. 1, pp. 91-100, 2013.

[144] A. A. Coulter, D. Keller, J. J. Amberg, E. J. Bailey, and R. R. Goforth, "Phenotypic plasticity in the spawning traits of bigheaded carp (Hypophthalmichthys spp.) in novel ecosystems," Freshwater Biology, vol. 58, no. 5, pp. 1029-1037, 2013.

[145] M. T. Cook, S. S. Heppell, and T. S. Garcia, "Invasive bullfrog larvae lack developmental plasticity to changing hydroperiod," The Journal of Wildlife Management, vol. 77, no. 4, pp. 655-662, 2013.

[146] A. L. Liebl, A. W. Schrey, C. L. Richards, and L. B. Martin, "Patterns of DNA methylation throughout a range expansion of an introduced songbird," Integrative and Comparative Biology, vol. 53, no. 2, pp. 351-358, 2013.

[147] L. Chen, C. J. Tiu, S. Peng, and E. Siemann, "Conspecific plasticity and invasion: invasive populations of Chinese tallow (Triadica sebifera) have performance advantage over native populations only in low soil salinity," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 9, Article ID e74961, 2013.

[148] L. J. Lamarque, A. J. Porte, C. Eymeric, J. B. Lasnier, C. J. Lortie, and S. Delzon, "A test for pre-adapted phenotypic plasticity in the invasive tree Acernegundo L," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 9, Article ID e74239, 2013.

[149] J. R. Moroney, P. W. Rundel, and V. L. Sork, "Phenotypic plasticity and differentiation in fitness-related traits in invasive populations of the mediterranean forb centaurea melitensis (Asteraceae)," The American Journal of Botany, vol. 100, no. 10, pp. 2040-2051, 2013.

[150] S. Yavno and M. G. Fox, "Morphological change and phenotypic plasticity in native and non-native pumpkinseed sunfish in response to sustained water velocities," Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 26, no. 11, pp. 2383-2395, 2013.

[151] S. Paudel and L. L. Battaglia, "Germination responses of the invasive Triadica sebifera and two co-occurring native woody species to elevated salinity across a Gulf Coast transition ecosystem," Wetlands, vol. 33, no. 3, pp. 527-535, 2013.

[152] H. Skalova, V. Jarosik, S. Dvorackova, and P. Pysek, "Effect of intra- and interspecific competition on the performance of native and invasive species of impatiens under varying levels of shade and moisture," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 5, Article ID e62842, 2013.

[153] H. Niu, L. Zhao, and J. Sun, "Phenotypic plasticity of reproductive traits in response to food availability in invasive and native species of nematode," Biological Invasions, vol. 15, no. 7, pp. 1407-1415, 2013.

[154] C. Coccia, P. Calosi, L. Boyero, A. J. Green, and D. T. Bilton, "Does ecophysiology determine invasion success? A comparison between the invasive boatman Trichocorixa verticalis verticalis and the native Sigara lateralis (Hemiptera: Corixidae) in South-West Spain," PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 5, Article ID e63105, 2013.

[155] A. M. Derry, A. M. Kestrup, and A. P. Hendry, "Possible influences of plasticity and genetic/maternal effects on species coexistence: native Gammarus fasciatus facing exotic amphipods," Functional Ecology, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 1212-1223, 2013.

[156] X. G. Wang, B. N. Hogg, K. Levy, and K. M. Daane, "Predicting the outcomes of a tri-trophic interaction between an indigenous parasitoid and an exotic herbivorous pest and its host plants," Annals of Applied Biology, vol. 163, no. 2, pp. 288-297, 2013.

[157] E. Pujol-Buxo, O. S. Sebastian, N. Garriga, and G. A. Llorente, "How does the invasive/native nature of species influence tadpoles' plastic responses to predators?" Oikos, vol. 122, no. 1, pp. 19-29, 2013.

[158] S. C. Cook-Patton and A. A. Agrawal, "Relatedness predicts phenotypic plasticity in plants better than weediness," Evolutionary Ecology Research, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 527-542, 2011.

[159] K. Palacio-Lopez and E. Gianoli, "Invasive plants do not display greater phenotypic plasticity than their native or non-invasive counterparts: a meta-analysis," Oikos, vol. 120, no. 9, pp. 1393-1401, 2011.

[160] C. E. Wainwright and E. E. Cleland, "Exotic species display greater germination plasticity and higher germination rates than native species across multiple cues," Biological Invasions, vol. 15, no. 10, pp. 2253-2264, 2013.

[161] E. M. Wolkovich, T. Jonathan Davies, H. Schaefer et al., "Temperature-dependent shifts in phenology contribute to the success of exotic species with climate change," The American Journal of Botany, vol. 100, no. 7, pp. 1407-1421, 2013.

[162] S. L. Chown, S. Slabber, M. A. McGeoch, C. Janion, and H. P. Leinaas, "Phenotypic plasticity mediates climate change responses among invasive and indigenous arthropods," Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 274, no. 1625, pp. 2531-2537, 2007.

Matthew R. J. Morris

Department of Biological Sciences, Universityof Calgary, 2500 University Drive NW, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N1N4

Correspondence should be addressed to Matthew R. J. Morris; mrjmorri@ucalgary.ca

Received 18 July 2014; Revised 29 September 2014; Accepted 30 September 2014; Published 15 October 2014

Academic Editor: Hirohisa Kishino

TABLE 1: Five important questions pertaining to PMP and invasive
success, and references pertaining to these questions published
in 2013.

Organism            Description            Reference

Question 1: Are invasive species plastic for
functional traits?

Cyanobacteria       Growth rate and          [131]
                    morphology were
                    altered by
                    temperature.

Plant               Physiological            [132]
                    plasticity permitted
                    savannah-adapted
                    trees to survive
                    floodwaters
                    throughout their
                    invaded range.

Plant               Climate variation        [133]
                    induced plasticity
                    in several
                    phenotypes.

Plant               Different growth         [134]
                    strategies in
                    different habitats
                    kept population
                    growth stable.

Plant               Reciprocal               [135]
                    transplant of 15
                    invasive populations
                    showed that all
                    populations were
                    similarly plastic.

Plant               Plasticity was           [136]
                    induced by water
                    depth and light
                    quality.

Plant               Different                [137]
                    populations of
                    invasive species
                    differed in
                    plasticity to
                    changing water
                    conditions.

Plant               Growth was altered       [138]
                    by nitrogen
                    concentrations.

Mollusc             Size-at-maturity         [139]
                    changed with
                    temperature,
                    permitted survival
                    during El Nino.

Mollusc             Shell shape              [140]
                    plasticity induced
                    by water flow
                    velocity.

Crustacean          Reproductive             [141]
                    plasticity detected
                    as facultative
                    parthenogenesis.

Insect              Acclimation to cool      [142]
                    temperatures
                    increased
                    performance.

Insect              Physiological            [143]
                    plasticity enabled
                    salt tolerance in
                    invaded island
                    habitats.

Fish                Plasticity found in      [144]
                    length of spawning
                    season.

Amphibian           Hydroperiod did not      [145]
                    affect growth or
                    development (no
                    plasticity
                    detected).

Bird                Epigenetic               [146]
                    modifications higher
                    in populations with
                    less genetic
                    diversity.

Question 2: Are invasive species as or more invasive
than their ancestors?

Plant               Invasive populations     [147]
                    more plastic than
                    populations from the
                    ancestral range.

Plant               8 invasive               [148]
                    populations were as
                    plastic as 8
                    populations from the
                    ancestral range for
                    20 highly plastic
                    traits.

Plant               2 invasive               [149]
                    populations had
                    evolved increased
                    and decreased
                    plasticity for
                    different traits, in
                    comparison to 18
                    populations from the
                    ancestral range.

Plant               Plasticity increased     [48]
                    in the invasive
                    population relative
                    to their resurrected
                    ancestors.

Fish                Invasive populations     [150]
                    were less plastic
                    than populations
                    from their ancestral
                    range.

Question 3: Is plasticity higher in invasive species than
in the competitors they are displacing?

Plant               Germination of           [151]
                    invasive species was
                    not affected by
                    salinity, presumably
                    implying
                    physiological
                    plasticity; one
                    native species
                    performed even
                    better.

Plant               Invasive species         [152]
                    were more plastic
                    than native species
                    and were better
                    competitors, but
                    this varied with the
                    invasive success
                    ofthe species.

Nematode            Plasticity in the        [153]
                    reproductive traits
                    of an invasive
                    species gave it a
                    competitive
                    advantage.

Insect              Physiological            [154]
                    plasticity to
                    temperature was
                    higher in an
                    invasive species.

Question 4: Is plasticity higher in invasive species than
in noninvasive species?

Insect              An invasive species      [45]
                    with a large range
                    was compared to an
                    invasive species
                    with a small range
                    on the same island;
                    the large-range
                    species was more
                    resistant to
                    temperature,
                    implying
                    physiological
                    plasticity.

Question 5: Does plasticity permit persistence of native
species in the face of invaders? (native/invader)

Crustacean/         Invaders were only       [155]
Crustacean          present in ion-rich
                    waters, natives in
                    ion-poor and ion-
                    rich waters.
                    Plasticity in
                    natives allowed ion-
                    poor populations to
                    migrate to ion-rich
                    waters,
                    supplementing a
                    dwindling ion-rich
                    native population.

Insect/Insect       Parasitoid wasps         [156]
Amphibian/Insect,   ably preyed upon an
Fish, Crustacean    invading moth,
                    irrespective of
                    moth's host plant.

                    Both native and          [157]
                    invasive amphibians
                    exhibited
                    behavioural and/or
                    morphological
                    plasticity in the
                    face of both native
                    and invasive
                    predators, although
                    the magnitude of the
                    plastic response was
                    smaller towards
                    invasive predators.

TABLE 2: Important review papers, meta-analyses, or large-scale
experiments on plasticity and invasive success.

Species       Number of species      Overall results        Reference
type

Plants        79 native-invasive     Trend for greater        [49]
              species comparisons    plasticity in
                                     invaders but better
                                     performance in
                                     natives; plasticity
                                     favoured better
                                     performance in
                                     disturbed
                                     environments.

Plants        5 native-invasive      Trend of higher          [50]
              species comparisons    plasticity for
                                     invaders, under
                                     resource limitation.

Plants        10                     In 6 of 10 cases,        [47]
              invasive-ancestral     invaders were more
              population             plastic than their
              comparisons            progenitors.

Plants        7 native-invasive      Species relatedness      [158]
              species comparisons    was a better
                                     predictor of
                                     plasticity than
                                     invasiveness.

Plants        75 invasive-native     Invasive species         [54]
              or                     were on average more
              invasive-noninvasive   plastic, but this
              species comparisons    was not always
                                     associated with a
                                     fitness benefit.

Plants        35 invasive-native     Invasive species         [159]
              or                     were, on average, as
              invasive-noninvasive   plastic as their
              conspecific species    conspecifics.
              pairs

Plants        211 species with       The most widespread      [44]
              different levels of    invasive species
              invasiveness           were also the most
                                     plastic, increasing
                                     biomass with
                                     resource abundance.

Plants        12 invasive and 12     Invaders on average      [160]
              native species in      displayed both
              shrub community        robustness to poor
                                     environments and
                                     increased plasticity
                                     under favourable
                                     environments.

Plants        330 invasive and 959   On average invaders      [161]
              native flowering       had shifted their
              plants                 flowering time with
                                     climate change while
                                     natives had not.

Insects       2 invasive and 4       No difference in the     [162]
              native species         extent of
                                     plasticity, but
                                     natives performed
                                     better under cool
                                     acclimation and
                                     Invaders performed
                                     better under warm
                                     acclimation.

Vertebrates   ???                    An extensive review      [77]
                                     on the ways species
                                     have coped with
                                     urban environments,
                                     including
                                     behavioural
                                     plasticity.

Birds         39 successfully and    Larger relative          [118]
              unsuccessfully         brain size
              introduced species     associated with
                                     invasive success.

Birds         69 species, 501        Larger relative          [119]
              introduction           brain size
              attempts               associated with
                                     invasive success.

Birds         196 species, 646       Larger relative          [120]
              introduction           brain size
              attempts               associated with
                                     increased innovation
                                     and invasive
                                     success.

Birds         202 species, 832       Larger relative          [122]
              introduction           brain size and
              attempts               broader ecological
                                     niches associated
                                     with invasive
                                     success.

Mammals       100 species, 513       Relative brain size      [121]
              introduction           important predictor
              attempts               ofinvasive success.

TABLE 3: Ways in which plasticity may facilitate or hamper
ecological speciation.

Process of      Plasticity              Plasticity
ecological      facilitates             hinders
speciation      speciation              speciation

Colonizing      PMP                     Maladaptive
divergent                               plasticity
environments    (i) Preadapted
                plasticity

                (ii) Cryptic
                genetic variation

Divergent       Production of           Migration and
selection on    divergent               postdispersal
divergent       phenotypes              plasticity erode
phenotypes in                           genetic
divergent       (i) Adaptive            differentiation
environments    plasticity

                (ii) Innovation/
                novelty

                (iii) Phenotypic
                accommodation

                Production of
                genetic variation

                (i) Mutations in
                conditionally
                expressed genes

                (ii) More targets
                for mutation

                Coincident
                variation: genetic
                assimilation

                (i) Modified            Divergent phenotypes
                phenotype is            in divergent
                adaptive                environments,
                                        generated by
                (ii) Plasticity         plasticity, weaken
                then lost via neutral   selection in the
                mutations or            new environment
                selection to reduce
                costs (modern
                definition)

                Correlated variation

                (i) Enhance the
                modified trait

                (ii) Improvement of
                traits "saved" by
                plasticity

                (iii) Novel selection
                imposed by changes
                to phenotypic
                correlations

Reproductive    Plastic changes         Migration and
isolation as    isolate populations     postdispersal
a byproduct     if plasticity is        plasticity erode
of selection    irreversible and        genetic
                assortative             differentiation
                mating occurs

                Genetic assimilation/   Plasticity in
                correlated variation    sexually selected
                leads to isolation      traits removes
                (e.g., reduced          reproductive
                hybrid fitness)         barriers (hybrid
                                        swarms)
COPYRIGHT 2014 COPYRIGHT 2010 SAGE-Hindawi Access to Research
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2014 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Morris, Matthew R.J.
Publication:International Journal of Evolutionary Biology
Article Type:Report
Date:Jan 1, 2014
Words:14866
Previous Article:Diversification in monkeyflowers: an investigation of the effects of elevation and floral color in the genus Mimulus.
Next Article:DNA methylation, epigenetics, and evolution in vertebrates: facts and challenges.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2022 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters |