Printer Friendly

Parasite-induced changes in behavior and color make Gammarus pulex more prone to fish predation.

INTRODUCTION

Parasites with a complex life cycle involving several hosts are faced with problems of transmission from one host to the next. There is a wealth of solutions to this problem (e.g., Moore and Gotelli 1990, Moore 1995). Well known are the parasite-induced changes in the behavior of intermediate hosts of Acanthocephala, a group of parasitic worms in vertebrates, especially in fish and birds. The behavior of the intermediate hosts of Acanthocephala, arthropods, may show various changes when infected, including changes in activity, photoreaction, escape behavior, substrate color choice, and vertical distribution (e.g., Moore 1984, Poulin 1994). These behavioral changes increase the exposure of the intermediate host to the next host of the parasite.

The timing of behavioral changes was studied in one system, that of Gammarus lacustris infected with Polymorphus paradoxus. Changes in behavior coincided with the onset of infectivity of the parasite for the next host (Bethel and Holmes 1973, 1974). There is some evidence that the behavioral changes in G. lacustris are caused by parasite-induced modifications in serotonergic neuromodulatory activity (Harris-Warrick et al. 1989, Helluy and Holmes 1990, Thompson and Kavaliers 1994). Intermediate hosts that harbor the infective stage of acanthocephalans, the cystacanth stage, may show physiological changes such as reduced [O.sub.2] consumption (Rumpus and Kennedy 1974) and increased hemocyanin concentration, the respiratory pigment for most crustaceans (Bentley and Hurd 1993).

Several studies have shown increased predation of intermediate hosts that are infected by acanthocephalans (reviewed by Moore 1984 and Nickol 1985). It is tempting to ascribe this increased predation risk to parasite-induced changes in behavior that make the intermediate host more vulnerable. Although cystacanths of most Acanthocephala species are colorless, some of those using aquatic crustaceans as intermediate hosts are bright orange colored, or cause changes in the pigmentation of their intermediate hosts (Nickol 1985). Almost all cases (five out of six) for which an increase of predation risk of infected intermediate hosts was shown concern aquatic crustaceans with marked color effects of their acanthocephalan parasites: Gammarus lacustris infected by Polymorphus paradoxus (Holmes and Bethel 1972, Bethel and Holmes 1977) or by Polymorphus minutus (Hindsbo 1972); Hyalella azteca infected by Corynosoma constrictum (Bethel and Holmes 1977); Gammarus pulex infected by Pomphorhynchus laevis (Kennedy et al. 1978); and Asellus intermedius infected by Acanthocephalus dirus (Camp and Huizinga 1979). In all these systems, the parasite not only induces behavioral changes, but at the same time increases the conspicuousness of the intermediate hosts by color effects.

It is well established that conspicuously colored prey suffer enhanced predation, called oddity selection (e.g., Curio 1976). In the interpretation of increased predation risk of intermediate hosts infected by acanthocephalans, parasite-induced behavioral changes may thus be confounded by parasite-induced changes in appearance. There has been one attempt to experimentally separate these causes. Bethel and Holmes (1977) painted oval marks, about the size and color of cystacanths of P. paradoxus, on the carapaces of uninfected G. lacustris. They were exposed to Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos) together with equal numbers of uninfected but unpainted prey. Unfortunately, Mallards are surface feeders and did not appear to feed actively on the gammarids. It is therefore not surprising that the proportions of marked and unmarked gammarids that were eaten were not different.

In our study, we used a different acanthocephalan system: Pomphorhynchus laevis, which uses Gammarus pulex as intermediate host. Three-spined sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus, a visually hunting fish, served as predator. The aim of our study was to disentangle the effects of parasite color and changed host behavior on the predation risk of G. pulex infected with P. laevis. The separate effects of color and behavior were studied experimentally by painting an orange spot that simulated infection on the cuticle of uninfected G. pulex, and covering the place through which the orange parasite was visible in infected G. pulex with a neutral color, respectively.

METHODS

The prey Gammarus pulex and the parasite Pomphorhynchus laevis

Gammarus pulex (Amphipoda) that were uninfected or visibly infected by the spiny-headed worm Pomphorhynchus laevis (Acanthocephala) were freshly collected with a long hand net for each set of the experiments (24 September 1995-09 October 1995). They were sampled from the Wohlensee near Berne, Switzerland (46 [degrees] 57 [minutes] N, 7 [degrees] 28 [minutes] E), at sites where sticklebacks had been breeding during April-July 1995. After collection, they were acclimatized over a few hours to room temperature. Only G. pulex of a total length between 0.5 and 1.0 cm were used in the experiments. Infected G. pulex are easily recognized, because the yellow-orange cystacanth stage of P. laevis is clearly visible through the cuticle of the intermediate host. The prevalence of infection in G. pulex was assessed on 14 and 18 September 1995 at three different breeding sites (Eymatt, Aumatt, Hasli) which are a few hundred meters apart. G. pulex were always handled with insect forceps.

In part of the experiments, an orange or brown spot of about the size of a single P. laevis cystacanth was painted on the cuticle of G. pulex. G. pulex were dabbed on absorbing tissue to remove attached water, picked up with an insect forceps, and a spot of paint consisting of about a 1:1 mixture of oil paint ("Marie's oil colours," Shanghai, China; colors "orange" and "raw sienna") and Tipp-Ex (a white typographic correction fluid manufactured by the Tipp-Ex Company of Frankfurt, Germany) (P. I. Ward, personal communication) was applied with a toothpick. The paint dried quickly after blowing. The whole procedure took [less than]2 min. The painted G. pulex were put in a beaker with water at room temperature and allowed at least 15 min to recover before they were offered as prey to sticklebacks. The particular oil colors were selected on the basis of matching the hue of the parasite color and the gammarid body color, respectively, although both oil colors had a somewhat greater brightness.

In order to check whether G. pulex in our population showed a changed photoreaction when infected by cystacanths of P. laevis, we used a similar setup to that used by Kennedy et al. (1978). The distribution of G. pulex in response to light was quantified in clear water in a small plastic tank (32.5 x 17.5 cm, water level 14 cm) providing a choice between light and dark zones. The bottom and sides as well as the top of one half of the tank were covered with opaque dark gray plastic. The tank was divided in two halves by an opaque, dark gray, plastic partition positioned 4 cm above the bottom of the tank. The uncovered half was lit by a 33 W fluorescent tube mounted 10 cm above the water surface. The light intensity was 3630 1x, 3 cm above the water surface. The tank was filled with Wohlensee water at room temperature, which was exchanged after every second test and aerated between but not during tests.

G. pulex were caught in July 1996 from the Wohlensee, 2 d before the tests. Infected and uninfected gammarids were stored separately in buckets filled with aerated Wohlensee water at room temperature and leaf litter. The gammarids were used only once. Ten infected and 10 uninfected G. pulex were put into a petri dish with water, which was placed at the bottom in the middle of the tank. They were allowed to leave the petri dish, after which it was removed. After 1 h of acclimatization, the numbers of infected and uninfected gammarids in the light half of the tank were recorded at 5-min intervals for 30 min. The ratio of total numbers of infected or uninfected gammarids in light to those in dark was used in the analysis. At the end of the experiments, the water temperature in the light half was 0.1 [degrees] C higher than in the dark half (mean [+ or -] 1 SD was 19.0 [degrees] [+ or -] 0.5 [degrees] and 18.9 [degrees] 0.5 [degrees] C, respectively).

The predator Gasterosteus aculeatus

Sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus, which were used as predators in the experiments, had been caught in the Wohlensee shortly before the start of the 1995 breeding season. P. laevis is the prevalent parasite in this stickleback population: about 75% of the sticklebacks are infected (T. C. M. Bakker and B. Mundwiler, unpublished data). The sticklebacks were stocked in a group of several hundred fish in an outside storage tank of [approximately equal to]200 L. The temperature in the tank was kept below 20 [degrees] C by a continuous inflow of cool water (10 [degrees]-15 [degrees] C) from a well. Day length was artificially prolonged to 16 h. The fish were fed frozen blood worms and live Tubifex worms.

The stored sticklebacks had not experienced gammarids for more than 6 mo and were thus inexperienced in catching G. pulex. Because gammarids are relatively difficult prey to catch, experience will influence the ease with which these prey are caught (e.g., Ibrahim and Huntingford 1992, Mackney and Hughes 1995). As the G. pulex used in the experiments will have been exposed to fish predators in the field, the combination of inexperienced predators and experienced prey will probably maximize differential predation.

Female sticklebacks that were not reproductively active and around 5 cm standard length were used in the experiments in order to reduce the effects of confounding variables on prey selection. They were used only once. In order to standardize the hunger level of the sticklebacks and to make them willing to eat gammarids, the sticklebacks were not fed for 3 d before the start of the experiments. They were acclimatized for a few hours to room temperature before they were introduced into the experimental tanks. The standard length and wet mass of the fish were assessed shortly before introduction. After the experiments the wet mass was measured again. The fish were given at least 1 h to become familiar with the experimental tank before the start of the experiment.

Experiments

In a pilot experiment, we determined predation rates under the conditions later used in the main experiments. The fish consumed about one G. pulex every 9 h. The duration of the experiments was therefore set at 47 h, so that most sticklebacks would not consume more than half of the 20 prey items offered.

The experiments were performed in small plastic tanks (35 x 18 cm, water level 13 cm). The outer walls of the tanks were covered with gray, opaque partitions to prevent interactions between fish in neighboring tanks. The tanks were filled with tap water that was renewed every second experiment. The water was at room temperature (18 [degrees]-21 [degrees] C) at the start of the experiment. Each tank was aerated through an airstone except during the experiment. During the experiment the airstone was removed, because G. pulex that clung to the white stone were easily detected. Each row of six tanks was lit by a 33 W fluorescent tube, mounted 37 cm above the water surface. The light:dark schedule was 14L:10D. Each tank was provided in its center with a Vallisneria plant planted in a plastic cup (height 7 cm, diameter 5 cm) filled with coarse gravel, and at the bottom with an almost black tree leaf of roughly 14 [cm.sup.2] that we had collected from leaf litter in the Wohlensee. The plant, gravel, and leaf offered G. pulex hiding places and food.

At least 1 h after the introduction of hungry fish into the experimental tanks, one in each tank, water containing 20 selected G. pulex was poured into each tank in such a way that the gammarids were divided over the whole tank. During the 47 h of the experiment the tanks were left undisturbed.

We performed three kinds of experiments in order to study the effects of parasite color and changed host behavior on the predation risk of G. pulex infected with P. laevis. In the first experiment, 10 infected and 10 uninfected G. pulex prey were exposed to sticklebacks. Gammarids in the two prey classes were matched for size by eye. Differential predation on the two prey classes can be due to effects of parasite color and/or host behavior. These two effects were experimentally separated in two further experiments.

The second experiment investigated the effect of parasite color on the predation risk of its intermediate host. Sticklebacks were again offered 20 G. pulex. These were all uninfected by P. laevis cystacanths. In half of the gammarids, an infection was simulated by painting dorsally on the cuticle of G. pulex an orange spot of similar color and size as a P. laevis cystacanth. The other half of the prey were treated similarly, but with brown paint that matched the body color of G. pulex. They served as a control for the effects of the paint. Gammarids in the two prey classes were matched for size by eye. Possibly differential effects of the two colors on G. pulex were checked in a pilot experiment. The fate of 20 uninfected G. pulex was assessed after 47 h in the experimental tank, but without a predator. They were either all painted with an orange or a brown spot. The mortality rate was equal for gammarids treated with the different colors (1 out of 20), as was the loss of the paint (4 of out 20), which was due to molting in half of the cases.

The third experiment investigated the effect of parasite-induced changes in host behavior on predation risk. Sticklebacks were again offered 20 G. pulex. Half of them were infected, but the parasite was masked by applying brown paint dorsally and laterally on the area where the cystacanth was visible. The other half of the prey were uninfected but treated similarly, and served as a control for effects of the paint. Gammarids in the two prey classes were matched for size by eye. Forty-seven hours after introduction of the prey, sticklebacks were removed and the tanks carefully checked for remaining prey.

Cases in which sticklebacks did not eat a single prey (6 times out of 70) and those in which more than half of the prey items were consumed (15 times out of 70) were left out of the analyses of predation rate. The actual number of infected prey eaten was compared with the expected number of infected prey eaten when sticklebacks do not discriminate. The expected numbers were corrected for the numbers of dead prey that we found at the end of the experiments, because sticklebacks do not eat dead prey items. The number of prey items that had not been consumed in Experiment 2 and that had lost their paint (a total of 37 prey items out of 233 left in 15 experiments) were assumed to be [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] equally divided among the two classes of prey (orange and brown).

RESULTS

G. pulex infected by P. laevis showed a changed photoreaction: in behavioral tests without predators, infected gammarids were significantly less often in the dark half of the tank than uninfected ones (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test on the ratio of light to dark per replicate, N = 7, T = 1, P [less than] 0.05, two-tailed). Median ratios of numbers in light to those in dark were 0.67 (range 0.37-1.12) and 0.37 (range 0.27-0.79) for infected and uninfected G. pulex, respectively.

Sticklebacks consumed significantly more infected G. pulex than uninfected ones (Experiment 1 in Table 1). Experimental exclusion of behavioral (Experiment 2) or color effects (Experiment 3) of the parasite on its intermediate host showed that both parasite color and parasite-induced changes in G. pulex behavior significantly increased their vulnerability to predation by sticklebacks (Table 1).

The proportion of prey eaten that was infected (naturally or simulated) was different among experiments (median proportion in Experiments 1, 2, and 3 was 0.65 (range 0-1), 1.0 (range 0.5-1), and 0.69 (range 0-1), respectively (Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA, df = 2, H = 6.82, P = 0.033). A multiple comparison between experiments showed that Experiment 1 and 2 differed significantly at the 5% level in this respect.

Infected G. pulex suffered higher mortality during the experiments than uninfected ones. At the end of Experiment 1, in which unpainted gammarids were [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 2 OMITTED] used, the median number of dead G. pulex was 1 (range 0-4) and 0 (range 0-3) for infected and uninfected gammarids, respectively (all replicates including those in pilot experiments and those not used in further analyses, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, N = 22, z = 3.22, P = 0.0013, two-tailed). In Experiment 3, in which brown paint was applied, the median numbers were 1 (range 0-5) and 0 (range 0-1), respectively (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, N = 9, T = 4.5, P = 0.033, two-tailed). The total number of gammarids that died in Experiments 1 (unpainted gammarids) and 3 (painted gammarids) was not significantly different (Mann Whitney U test, N = 13 and 34, z = 1.47, P = 0.14, two-tailed).

The fish used in the different experiments had a comparable body size, body mass, and condition factor (Table 2). The total number of G. pulex eaten differed among experiments and therefore so did the mass lost by sticklebacks (Table 2). In Experiment 1, more prey were eaten and the fish lost less mass than in Experiments 2 and 3 (post-hoc Fisher PLSD tests on the number of prey eaten, 1 vs. 2 P [less than] 0.05, 1 vs. 3 and 2 vs. 3 P [greater than] 0.05; Fisher PLSD test on mass loss, 1 vs. 2 and 1 vs. 3 P [less than] 0.05, 2 vs. 3 P [greater than] 0.05). These results suggest that painted prey were less attractive to sticklebacks. The results were similar when the total number of prey eaten and the mass loss were standardized for differences in fish body size using residual values from linear regression.

The prevalence of P. laevis infection in G. pulex in the Wohlensee, as determined shortly before the experiments, was 11.8% (N = 356), 7.8% (N = 655), and 20.8% (N = 424) at the sites Eymatt, Aumatt, and Hasli, respectively; the mean prevalence being 14.4% (N = 1435). A sample of 214 G. pulex caught at the same sites in September 1993 showed a similar prevalence (i.e., 19.6%). The intensity of infection (mean number of P. laevis per G. pulex) in this sample was 1.29; 73.8% (N = 31) had one, 23.8% (N = 10) had two, and 2.4% (N = 1) had three cystacanths.

DISCUSSION

We have experimentally shown for the first time that parasite-induced changes in behavior as well as coloration promoted the transmission of the acanthocephalan Pomphorhynchus laevis from its intermediate host Gammarus pulex to its next host, the three-spined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus. Our experiments cannot give information about the relative extent of these two causes. The paint that we used to mimic cystacanths of P. laevis and to hide visible orange cystacanths, respectively, worked well in the sense that it did not seem to affect the gammarids' behavior or survival. Although we tried to match the parasite color and gammarids' body color as well as possible, natural colors are variable. Additionally, the odor and taste of painted and unpainted gammarids may have been different. Sticklebacks ate somewhat less of the painted prey. However, they found the gammarids with simulated infection more attractive than naturally infected gammarids. For these reasons, the magnitude of the effects of parasite color and changed host behavior on the susceptibility of gammarids to fish predation cannot be compared. Additionally, the ratio of infected prey eaten to uninfected prey eaten in the various experiments slightly underestimates the actual degree of differential susceptibility, because the sticklebacks were sampling gammarids without replacement. The actual differential susceptibility depends on the experimental setup and performed manipulations, and may be different under natural conditions.

Why is P. laevis so conspicuously colored? Does this parasite need to give its intermediate host a more conspicuous appearance, in addition to behavioral changes, in order to guarantee its transmission to the next host? Or does the conspicuous orange color of the parasite have other functions? It may be significant that the orange cystacanths belong to Acanthocephala species which use aquatic crustaceans as intermediate hosts. In other aquatic parasite systems, intermediate hosts also undergo parasite-induced changes in behavior, but these are not accompanied by color effects of the parasite, e.g., Cyclops vernalis (Copepoda) infected by Eubothrium salvelini (Cestoda) (Poulin et al. 1992); and cyclopoid copepod species (Cyclops abyssorum and Macrocyclops albidus) infected by Schistocephalus solidus (Cestoda) (Urdal et al. 1995, Wedekind and Milinski 1996). In these cestode systems, the main parasite-induced change in behavior is an increase in swimming activity of the infected copepods. Poulin et al. (1992) could not detect differences in time spent near the surface of infected and uninfected copepods in a water column of 14 cm, but in Wedekind and Milinski's (1996) experiments, more infected copepods moved to the water surface than uninfected ones when transferred to a water column of only 2 cm. In a meta-analysis of parasite-induced behavioral changes, Poulin (1994) found that cestodes have a greater influence on host activity than acanthocephalans, while the reverse was true for host microhabitat choice.

For some of the intermediate hosts of orange Acanthocephala, the changes in photoreaction as a result of parasitization have been studied (reviewed in Nickol 1985). Uninfected Gammarus lacustris, Hyalella azteca, and Gammarus pulex are photophobic and negatively phototactic, but infection with various orange Acanthocephala species reversed their photoreaction: they became photophilic and positively phototactic. We found similar changes in G. pulex infected by P. laevis. The exception is G. lacustris infected by Polymorphus marilis: the host became photophilic but stayed negatively phototactic (Bethel and Holmes 1973, 1977). However, the main host for this parasite is a diving bird, the Lesser Scaup. Thus, the behavioral changes may be adequate for transmission of P. marilis. There is one obvious consequence of the changes in photoreaction induced by the orange Acanthocephala species: an increase in exposure of their intermediate hosts and themselves to the deleterious effects of solar UV-B radiation (280-320 nanometers) (e.g., Williamson 1995).

The orange color of the above-mentioned Acanthocephala species is made up of carotenoids (e.g., Barrett and Butterworth 1968, 1973). Numerous beneficial metabolic and nutritional functions have been suggested for carotenoids (e.g., Goodwin 1986, Segner et al. 1989), one of these being photoprotection. In zooplankton there is convincing evidence that carotenoids protect against the damaging effects of UV-B radiation (e.g., Hairston 1976, 1979, 1980, Ringelberg 1980, Luecke and O'Brien 1981, Byron 1982, Ringelberg et al. 1984). Experiments showed that more pigmented zooplankton pay a cost in terms of increased predation by visually selective predators (e.g., Byron 1982). The orange color of some acanthocephalans may thus be an adaptation to cope with increased levels of UV-B radiation caused by parasite-induced changes in the behavior of their intermediate hosts, aquatic crustaceans. The additional benefit of orange cystacanth color to the enhancement of transmission to the next host may have further facilitated the evolution of conspicuous parasite coloration. It would be interesting to test this idea with comparative data of aquatic Acanthocephala systems. Are intermediate hosts of orange-colored acanthocephalans (or alternatively, parasite-induced dark colored intermediate hosts [see Nickol 1985]; melanin is another photoprotective pigment [Luecke and O'Brien 1983]) more exposed to UV-B radiation than intermediate hosts of colorless acanthocephalans (or alternatively, parasite-induced light colored intermediate hosts [caused by pigmentation dystrophy, see Nickol 1985])?

It would be an easy task for sticklebacks to avoid gammarids infected by P. laevis. Why don't they do so? Often there will be no selective pressure to avoid parasitized prey, because the benefits of ingesting parasitized prey are higher than the costs of avoiding them (Lafferty 1992). We showed that infected gammarids are more frequently preyed upon than uninfected ones. The parasite-induced changes in both prey behavior and coloration make them more profitable prey. Infected gammarids made up a significant proportion of the gammarid population in our field site, at least at some times of the year. Gammarids are beneficial to sticklebacks because they are an important source for carotenoids (e.g., Simpson et al. 1981, Boonyaratpalin and Unprasert 1989; T. C. M. Bakker and B. Mundwiler, personal observation). The orange-red breeding coloration of stickleback males, which is made up of carotenoids (e.g., Brush and Reisman 1965, Czeczuga 1980), plays an important role in sexual selection: redder males are preferred as mates by females (e.g., Milinski and Bakker 1990, Bakker 1993, Bakker and Mundwiler 1994) and have greater chances to establish territories (e.g., Bakker and Sevenster 1983). G. pulex infected with P. laevis showed increased mortality under our laboratory conditions. There are conflicting reports about the damaging effects of this parasite in fish (e.g., Chubb 1965, Hine and Kennedy 1974). In small fish species like the stickleback, it is likely that the parasite reduces the fitness of its host: the intensity of infection in sticklebacks found dead in the field was greater than that in sticklebacks which were reproductively active at the same time (T. C. M. Bakker and B. Mundwiler, personal observation). Additionally, the parasite affects physical condition and secondary sexual traits (eye color and relative pectoral fin size) in reproductively active males (T. C. M. Bakker and B. Mundwiler, personal observation), thereby probably decreasing reproductive success. On the other hand, sticklebacks of our Wohlensee population reproduce only during one breeding season and then die. Thus, they have to cope with the negative effects of P. laevis only for a short period of time.

In our experiments, there was individual variation in the extent to which infected gammarids were preferentially eaten. We have indications that there exists genetic variation in the susceptibility to P. laevis in Wohlensee sticklebacks (T. C. M. Bakker and B. Mundwiler, personal observation). It will be a challenge to link this to variation in prey choice.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Patrick Boltshauser and Reto Kunzler for assistance, Jean Mariaux for the determination of the acanthocephalan parasite, John C. Holmes and an anonymous referee for helpful comments, and the Swiss National Science Foundation for financial support.

LITERATURE CITED

Bakker, T. C. M. 1993. Positive genetic correlation between female preference and preferred male ornament in sticklebacks. Nature 363:255-257.

Bakker, T. C. M., and B. Mundwiler. 1994. Female mate choice and male red coloration in a natural stickleback population. Behavioral Ecology 5:74-80.

Bakker, T. C. M., and P. Sevenster. 1983. Determinants of dominance in male sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Behaviour 86:55-71.

Barrett, J., and P. E. Butterworth. 1968. The carotenoids of Polymorphus minutus (Acanthocephala) and its intermediate host, Gammarus pulex. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 27:575-581.

Barrett, J., and P. E. Butterworth. 1973. The carotenoid pigments of six species of adult Acanthocephala. Experientia 29:651-653.

Bentley, C. R., and H. Hurd. 1993. Pomphorhynchus laevis (Acanthocephala): elevation of haemolymph protein concentration in the intermediate host, Gammarus pulex (Crustacea: Amphipoda). Parasitology 107:193-198.

Bethel, W. M., and J. C. Holmes. 1973. Altered evasive behavior and responses to light in amphipods harboring acanthocephalan cystacanths. Journal of Parasitology 59: 945-956.

Bethel, W. M., and J. C. Holmes. 1974. Correlation of development of altered evasive behavior in Gammarus lacustris (Amphipoda) harboring cystacanths of Polymorphus paradox (Acanthocephala) with the infectivity to the definitive host. Journal of Parasitology 60:272-274.

Bethel, W. M., and J. C. Holmes. 1977. Increased vulnerability of amphipods to predation owing to altered behavior induced by larval acanthocephalans. Canadian Journal of Zoology 55:110-115.

Bolger, T., and P. L. Connolly. 1989. The selection of suitable indices for the measurement and analysis of fish condition. Journal of Fish Biology 34:171-182.

Boonyaratpalin, M., and N. Unprasert. 1989. Effects of pigments from different sources on colour changes and growth of red Oreochromis niloticus. Aquaculture 79:375-380.

Brush, A. H., and H. M. Reisman. 1965. The carotenoid pigments in the three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 14:121-125.

Byron, E. A. 1982. The adaptive significance of calanoid copepod pigmentation: a comparative and experimental analysis. Ecology 63:1871-1886.

Camp, J. W., and H. W. Huizinga. 1979. Altered color, behavior and predation susceptibility of the isopod, Asellus intermedius, infected with Acanthocephalus dirus. Journal of Parasitology 65:667-669.

Chubb, J. C. 1965. Mass occurrence of Pomphorhynchus laevis (Muller, 1776) Monticelli 1905 (Acanthocephala) in the chub Squalius cephalus L. from the River Avon, Hampshire. Parasitology 55:1-5.

Curio, E. 1976. The ethology of predation. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Germany.

Czeczuga, B. 1980. Carotenoids in fish. XXVI. Pungitius pungitius (L.) and Gasterosteus aculeatus L. (Gasterosteidae). Hydrobiologia 74:7-10.

Goodwin, T. W. 1986. Metabolism, nutrition, and function of carotenoids. Annual Review of Nutrition 6:273-297.

Hairston, N. G., Jr. 1976. Photoprotection by carotenoid pigments in the copepod Diaptomus nevadensis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 73:971-974.

-----. 1979. The relationship between pigmentation and reproduction in two species of Diaptomus (Copepoda). Limnology and Oceanography 24:38-44.

-----. 1980. The vertical distribution of diaptomid copepods in relation to body pigmentation. Pages 98-110 in W. C. Kerfoot, editor. Evolution and ecology of zooplankton communities. University Press of New England, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA.

Harris-Warrick, R. M., R. E. Flamm, B. R. Johnson, and P. S. Katz. 1989. Modulation of neural circuits in crustacea. American Zoologist 29:1305-1320.

Helluy, S., and J. C. Holmes. 1990. Serotonin, octapamine, and the clinging behavior induced by the parasite Polymorphus paradoxus (Acanthocephala) in Gammarus lacustris (Crustacea). Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:1214-1220.

Hindsbo, O. 1972. Effects of Polymorphus (Acanthocephala) on colour and behaviour of Gammarus lacustris. Nature 238:333.

Hine, P. M., and C. R. Kennedy. 1974. Observations on the distribution, specificity and pathogenicity of the acanthocephalan Pomphorhynchus laevis (Muller). Journal of Fish Biology 6:521-535.

Holmes, J. C., and W. M. Bethel. 1972. Modification of intermediate host behaviour by parasites. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society, Supplement 1 51:123-149.

Ibrahim, A. A., and F. A. Huntingford. 1992. Experience of natural prey and feeding efficiency in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Journal of Fish Biology 41:619-625.

Kennedy, C. R., P. F. Broughton, and P. M. Hine. 1978. The status of brown and rainbow trout, Salmo trutta and S. gairdneri as hosts of the acanthocephalan, Pomphorhynchus laevis. Journal of Fish Biology 13:265-275.

Lafferty, K. D. 1992. Foraging on prey that are modified by parasites. The American Naturalist 140:854-867.

Luecke, C., and W. J. O'Brien. 1981. Phototoxicity and fish predation: selective factors in color morphs in Heterocope. Limnology and Oceanography 26:454-460.

Luecke, C., and W. J. O'Brien. 1983. Photoprotective pigmentation in a pond morph of Daphnia middendorffiana. Arctic 36:365-368.

Mackney, P. A., and R. N. Hughes. 1995. Foraging behaviour and memory window in sticklebacks. Behaviour 132:1241-1253.

Milinski, M., and T. C. M. Bakker. 1990. Female sticklebacks use male coloration in mate choice and hence avoid parasitized males. Nature 344:330-333.

Moore, J. 1984. Altered behavioral responses in intermediate hosts - an acanthocephalan parasite strategy. American Naturalist 123:572-577.

-----. 1995. The behavior of parasitized animals. Bio-Science 45:89-96.

Moore, J., and N. J. Gotelli. 1990. A phylogenetic perspective on the evolution of altered host behaviours: a critical look at the manipulation hypothesis. Pages 193-233 in C. J. Barnard and J. M. Behnke, editors. Parasite and host behaviour. Taylor and Francis, London, UK.

Nickol, B. B. 1985. Epizootiology. Pages 307-346 in D. W. T. Crompton and B. B. Nickol, editors. Biology of the Acanthocephala. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Poulin, R. 1994. Meta-analysis of parasite-induced behavioral changes. Animal Behaviour 48:137-146.

Poulin, R., M. A. Curtis, and M. E. Rau. 1992. Effects of Eubothrium salvelini (Cestoda) on the behaviour of Cyclops vernalis (Copepoda) and its susceptibility to fish predators. Parasitology 105:265-271.

Ringelberg, J. 1980. Aspects of red pigmentation in zooplankton, especially copepods. Pages 91-97 in W. C. Kerfoot, editor. Evolution and ecology of zooplankton communities. University Press of New England, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA.

Ringelberg, J., A. L. Keyser, and B. J. G. Flik. 1984. The mortality effect of ultraviolet radiation in a translucent and in a red morph of Acanthodiaptomus denticornis (Crustacea, Copepoda) and its possible ecological relevance. Hydrobiologia 112:217-222.

Rumpus, A. E., and C. R. Kennedy. 1974. The effect of the acanthocephalan Pomphorhynchus laevis upon the respiration of its intermediate host, Gammarus pulex. Parasitology 68:271-284.

Segner, H., P. Arend, K. von Poeppinghausen, and H. Schmidt. 1989. The effect of feeding astaxanthin to Oreochromis niloticus and Colisa labiosa on the histology of the liver. Aquaculture 79:381-390.

Simpson, K. L., T. Katayama, and C. O. Chichester. 1981. Carotenoids in fish feeds. Pages 463-538 in J. C. Bauernfeind, editor. Carotenoids as colorants and vitamin A precursors. Technological and Nutritional Applications. Academic Press, New York, New York, USA.

Thompson, S. N., and M. Kavaliers. 1994. Physiological bases for parasite-induced alterations of host behaviour. Parasitology 109:S119-S138.

Urdal, K., J. F. Tierney, and P. J. Jakobsen. 1995. The tapeworm Schistocephalus solidus alters the activity and response, but not the predation susceptibility of infected copepods. Journal of Parasitology 81:330-333.

Wedekind, C., and M. Milinski. 1996. Do three-spined sticklebacks avoid consuming copepods, the first intermediate host of Schistocephalus solidus - an experimental analysis of behavioural resistance. Parasitology 112:371-383.

Williamson, C. E. 1995. What role does UV-B radiation play in freshwater ecosystems? Limnology and Oceanography 40:386-392.
COPYRIGHT 1997 Ecological Society of America
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1997 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Bakker, Theo C.; Mazzi, Dominique; Zala, Sarah
Publication:Ecology
Date:Jun 1, 1997
Words:5547
Previous Article:Prey abundance and predator reproduction: rats aand pythons on a tropical Australian floodplain.
Next Article:Constraints on frugivory by bears.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2019 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters