Printer Friendly

Nonbelievers' beliefs about religion in China.

Nonbelievers are individuals who lack any beliefs in a religion (http://bit.ly/1pw4dXG). Findings in studies have shown that the number of nonbelievers has significantly increased globally with the progress of modernization (Schjoedt, Stodkilde-Jorgensen, Geertz, & Roepstorff, 2009; Waytz, Gray, Epley, & Wegner, 2010; Zuckerman, 2008). In China in recent years, the younger generation of nonbelievers has paid increasing attention to religion (Han, 2003; Hua, 2009; Tan, 2001), despite the fact that it is in China that the world's highest concentration of nonbelievers is found (WIN-Gallup International, 2012). This raises the question of what beliefs about religion Chinese nonbelievers hold.

Little research has been focused on nonbelievers' beliefs about religion (Geertz & Markusson, 2010; Johnson, 2012), and this lack applies especially to Chinese nonbelievers. It was the lack of information that motivated my research in regard to three key points. First, beliefs about religion refer to what people know about religion, regardless of whether or not they are believers or nonbelievers. Nonbelievers, as well as believers, hold generalized beliefs about religion or views about how the spiritual world functions. Nonetheless, "lacking belief in religion" is commonly equated with "lacking belief about religion," which has obscured researchers' views of the nonbelievers' beliefs about religion. In many cases, researchers have excluded religious nonbelievers from participation in a study or have not sampled them in sufficient numbers to produce conclusions about this growing subgroup (Pargament, Koenig, Tarakeshwar, & Hahn, 2004). In order to gather accurate information regarding nonbelievers, I believe that it is essential for researchers to stop neglecting nonbelievers' beliefs about religion. What people "know" commonly guides how they act (Furnham, 1988), therefore determining Chinese nonbelievers' beliefs about religion is important and helpful with regard to predicting their decisions and behaviors when encountering religious situations.

Second, it is well known that there are differences between nonbelievers and believers in their beliefs about the existence of a Supreme Being and an afterlife (Owen, Hayward, Koenig, Steffen, & Payne, 2011; Singelis, Hubbard, Her, & An, 2003). In addition, there is a common misunderstanding that nonbelievers are the opposite of believers, thus resulting in a widespread negative bias, and even discrimination, against nonbelievers (Edgell, Gerteis, & Hartmann, 2006). When scholars have conducted scientific research on the subject of religion, nonbelievers have also often been used as the control group. However, the differences between believers and nonbelievers do not, of themselves, represent the nonbelievers' beliefs about religion in its entirety. In their generalized beliefs about religion some specific points of individual difference may exist among nonbelievers that would prove helpful in uncovering a more comprehensive picture regarding nonbelievers and their beliefs.

Researchers have provided some different viewpoints on nonbelievers' beliefs about religion, which have inspired my research. Nonbelievers have been identified with some specific positive consequences of religion, such as the moral framework (Moore, 2005) and psychological benefits (Koenig & Larson, 2001). Some nonbelievers reported being spiritual but not religious (Fuller, 2001), and some have reported that they struggled to find meaning in life (O'Connell & Skevington, 2010; Wilkinson & Coleman, 2010). In addition, some nonbelievers have even stated that they held beliefs in religion to a certain extent. "Believing without belonging" is a phrase that has been used to describe the disconnection of religious beliefs and affiliation (Davie, 1990), and this position has been well evidenced in Europe and North America (Davie, 2006; Glendinning & Bruce, 2006). Some nonbelievers have identified themselves as simply unsure religiously (Exline, Park, Smith, & Carey, 2011) or uncertain about life after death (James & Wells, 2002). Other nonbelievers have not completely rejected religion. For example, in one study the overwhelming majority of religious tourists and worshippers in China were found to be nonbelievers (Cao, 2003).

Third, beliefs about religion fluctuate according to social-cultural context and historical period (Gervais & Norenzayan, 2012), so that research about nonbelievers' beliefs about religion in a specific social-cultural context may broaden understanding of the nature of religious belief. In modern China, in the complex mix of social transformation (a specific historical period), predominant atheism, and traditional culture with strong religious leanings, studies on Chinese nonbelievers' beliefs about religion will provide valuable insights that will contribute toward better understanding of the psychology and behavior of nonbelievers.

My purpose in this study was to explore Chinese nonbelievers' beliefs about religion. As a significant vulnerability has been identified in religiosity between the ages of 18 and 30 years (Argue, Johnson, & White, 1999), in my study I focused on the younger generation of Chinese nonbelievers. As research on nonbelievers is scarce (Johnson, 2012), I adopted a broader perspective on nonbelievers and included all individuals who self-reported as not believing in any particular religion, in order to obtain a more comprehensive picture of nonbelievers.

Method

The Research Ethics Board at Beijing Youth Politics College approved this study. All participants provided written informed consent.

Participants

I collected data from a random sample of 638 Chinese young people studying at universities and graduate schools in Beijing. They came from 16 provinces of China (Beijing, Shanghai, Jilin, Jiangsu, Guangdong, Fujian, Hainan, Jiangxi, Neimeng, Ningxia, Shanxi, Shandong, Sichuan, Guizhou, Xinjiang, and Hubei).

Among them, 60 were believers (average age of 23.47 [+ or -] 3.13 with 25 women in this group) and 578 were nonbelievers (average age of 23.51 [+ or -] 3.30 with 310 women in the group). The proportion of believers (9.4%) and nonbelievers (90.6%) reflects the composition of religion in China today (WIN-Gallup International, 2012).

Among the believers' group, 28% were Christians, 30% were Buddhists, 6% were Taoists, and 36% were Muslims.

In terms of the education level of the believers, 58.3% were undergraduates, 25% were graduates, and 16.7% were postgraduates. Among the believers group, 73.3% were majoring in humanities and social sciences and 26.7% were majoring in natural sciences. Among the nonbelievers' group, 40.1% were undergraduates, 32.3% were graduates, and 27.6% were postgraduates, with 51.6% of the nonbelievers majoring in humanities and social sciences and 48.4% majoring in natural sciences.

Measures

Leung et al. (2002) developed a cross-cultural religiosity subscale of the Social Axioms Survey (SAS) in which the focus is on what people know about religion and its functions. A social axiom is a generalized belief that is central to a person's belief system, and its function is to enhance the survival and functioning of the person in his or her social and physical environment (Leung et al., 2002). The functional view helps explain why people do, or do not, choose to stay involved with a religion, with some staying involved even when they do not fully approve of the institution of religion and/or do not believe all of the religious claims or dogma.

Within a social axioms framework, religiosity refers to the view that spiritual forces influence personal life and that religious institutions exert a positive effect on social outcomes (Bond, Leung, Au, Tong, & Chemonges-Nielson, 2004). Religiosity is unrelated to specific religious beliefs and is, rather, based purely on a general belief about religion (Leung, Au, Huang, Kurman, Niit, & Niit, 2007), thus the scale is inclusive for all believers or nonbelievers. Religiosity of social axioms has been evidenced to be a good predictor of some social behaviors such as accommodation and competition in conflict resolution (Bond et al., 2004; Leung et al., 2002).

I used the religiosity subscale of the SAS to determine people's beliefs about religion. The scale consists of eight items (see Table 1), covering the most important personal and social functions of religion along with belief in the existence of a Supreme Being. The personal functions of religion in this scale include psychological benefits, a meaningful life, and reduction of disasters. In the social functions aspect are moral guidelines and education for citizenship (community spirit). Respondents answer on a 5-point Likert scale to rank the degree to which they believe each of the eight items to be true (1 = strongly disbelieve to 5 = strongly believe). The higher the score is, the more the respondent believes in the existence of a Supreme Being and the positive consequences of adherence to religion. The score of 3 is the midpoint and was, therefore, regarded as the cut-off point for disbelief and belief in this study. The internal consistency (Cronbach's coefficient alpha) for the religiosity subscale was .78 (Leung & Bond, 2004). The current reliability coefficient (Cronbach's coefficient alpha) was .71.

All participants self-described as either nonbelievers or believers when asked the following question: "Do you regard yourself as believing in any particular religion currently?" A single item measure of religiosity has commonly been used and has been well validated (Gebauer, Sedikides, & Neberich, 2012; Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006; WIN-Gallup International, 2012).

Procedure

I distributed the survey to all participants and collected the forms in class. All participants were instructed to supply demographic information about gender, age, and study major. In order to encourage honest responses, it was emphasized that all information would be used only as scientific research and all responses would be completely anonymous; it was explained that there were no right or wrong answers to any of the questions. At the end, I offered a gift of a water bottle, valued at US$3, as a reward to each participant. All of the experimenters were postgraduate students majoring in social psychology, and they were trained in the standardization of testing.

Analysis

I performed analysis at scale level and item level, respectively. The mean of the eight items was calculated as the religiosity scale score for each participant. Because each item represented one particular aspect of beliefs about religion, the single items were also analyzed to elaborate on the meaning of beliefs about religion. Descriptive statistics include religiosity scale score and each item score.

In order to gain a more comprehensive understanding of nonbelievers' beliefs on religion, I compared nonbelievers not only with believers but also with the midpoint (neutral). An independent sample t test was conducted for the comparison of believers' and nonbelievers' religiosity. One sample t test was conducted for the comparison between religiosity of nonbelievers and the midpoint of 3 (neutral). The data were analyzed using an alpha level of .05. All analyses were conducted using SPSS Version 15.0 for Windows.

Results

Descriptive statistics and t test for religiosity scale score and item scores are shown in Table 1. With regard to beliefs of these young Chinese students about religion, I divided the results into two parts: a) how nonbelievers differed from believers, and, b) how nonbelievers' score differed from the midpoint (neutral) of religiosity.

How Nonbelievers Differed From Believers with Regard to Beliefs About Religion

At scale level, religiosity among the believers' group was significantly higher than that of the nonbelievers' group.

At item level, there were similarities between the two groups in beliefs about the psychological benefits derived from religion; both nonbelievers and believers were neutral. The most notable difference between the two groups was that the nonbelievers' group disbelieved more in the statement that religion makes people good citizens (Cohen's d = -.53). The nonbelievers' group believed significantly less than the believers did in the existence of a Supreme Being, in the surreal functions of religion to do with the pursuit of good fortune and avoidance of disaster, and in both the spirituality and the moral framework functions of religion; whereas the nonbelievers' group believed significantly more than the believers did that religion makes people escape from reality and that religious beliefs lead to unscientific thinking.

How Nonbelievers' Scores Differed From the Midpoint (Neutral) of Religiosity

There was no statistically significant difference from the midpoint (neutral) in the religiosity scale score of the nonbelievers' group. This indicated that nonbelievers were middle of the road; their beliefs about religion were neutral.

Relative to the midpoint, the nonbelievers' group believed more in both the moral framework and the spirituality of religion, but they believed less in the surreal consequences, such as pursuing good fortune and avoiding disaster. For all other items, there was no statistically significant difference from the midpoint (neutral) for the nonbelievers' group.

Discussion

Chinese nonbelievers' beliefs about religion generally remain strikingly neutral despite being less strong than those of believers. On one hand, this result is consistent with findings in previous studies on the differences between believers and nonbelievers (Gebauer et al., 2012; James & Wells, 2002; Moore, 2005). On the other hand, in my study the puzzle of the beliefs of nonbelievers in China has been tentatively figured out in that the students who took part in this study adopted a neutral position on religion rather than taking a stance that was the extreme opposite of believers, and the nonbelievers' group even stood closer to the midpoint than the believers did with regard to the moral and spiritual functions of religion.

Religious belief is universally a pluralized construct rather than a dichotomy between belief and disbelief in nature (Chaves, 2010; Davie, 2006; McGuire, 2008; Storm, 2009). Researchers have developed a five-category model that they have termed The Five As in which different aspects of individual religious beliefs are combined and blended as follows: abiders, adapters, assenters, avoiders, and atheists (Pearce, Foster, & Halliday Hardie, 2013). Therefore, being neutral does not necessarily mean that religion has no impact on the younger generation of Chinese; being neutral is just another type of belief about religion among these nonbelievers.

As to why nonbelievers, who represent the majority group in China, take a neutral stand with regard to religion, Norenzayan, Gervais, and Trzesniewski (2012) suggested that beliefs about religion in nature to a certain extent reflect the social-cultural way of the living material life or spiritual life. The coincidence of Chinese religious culture with the development of religion propelled by modernization may provide an explanation as to the influences that lay behind the results obtained in my study.

Chinese religious culture has three typical characteristics. First, nontheistic naturalism and humanism figure more prominently in the history of Chinese religion than in the history of religion in the western world, although this has never been to the exclusion of theistic belief (Adler, 2002; Li, 2006; Lou, 2007; Zhou, 1985). In addition, in contemporary China, Marxism has again reinforced the nontheistic tradition.

Second, philosophy focused on spirituality is popular in Chinese daily life and has been ingrained in Chinese culture from ancient time up to the present day. Chinese people have not involved themselves as much with religion as they have been concerned with philosophy; it is through philosophy that they satisfy their craving for what is beyond the actual world and, thereby, practice self-cultivation (Fung, 1948). This philosophy has mapped a road for Chinese leading to discovery, spiritual development, and the nurturing and enhancing of their spiritual maturity, which provides a similar spiritual function to that of religion. In this study the neutral result implied that spiritual, but not religious, beliefs may exist among these nonbelievers. In western societies, spirituality is not necessarily viewed as being rooted in religion (Miller & Thoresen, 2003) and spiritual people have a loving, forgiving, and nonjudgmental view of the numinous (Hill et al., 2000). This is the same as self-cultivation ("Xiu Shen Yang Xing" is the Chinese phrase meaning to behave properly to cultivate oneself), which is the philosophy that is the basis of the life philosophy (Xiu Xing) of most Chinese people.

Third, Chinese religions have absorbed and fused so deeply with traditional Chinese philosophy (Lou, 1994; Xu, 2013) that the general Chinese population does not consciously deal with each on its separate merits. This fusion may be the reason that the nonbelievers who took part in my study did not belong to any particular religion but significantly believed in some positive consequences of religion.

Development of religion driven by modernization leads to deity fading (Inglehart & Baker, 2000; McGrath, 2004; Norris & Inglehart, 2004) with spirituality highlighted (Hogan, 2010; Inglehart & Baker, 2000) and this has coincided with both the nontheistic religious tradition and the life philosophy focused on spirituality that are embedded in Chinese culture. This coincidence may increase the sense of identity to religion and may have contributed to the neutral beliefs about religion among Chinese nonbelievers, which may explain why the Chinese nonbelievers in my study paid attention to, and did not reject, religion.

The practical implications of my findings are clear. I have contributed to uncovering the truth about nonbelievers' beliefs, to dispelling the suspicions Chinese people have about nonbelief, and to generating more, and more meaningful, dialogue between believers and nonbelievers.

Thus the data from this study highlight one true aspect of nonbelievers. Further research should be conducted to replicate the current findings and the self-reported single-item measure of a believer or a nonbeliever should be improved to better control for the spirituality of nonbelievers.

In conclusion, individuals in this study who did not belong to any particular religious group in China generally remained neutral about beliefs about religion. Among my participant group, nonbelievers were not the absolute antithesis of believers nor was the nonbelievers' group purely a control group in a scientific research exercise about a believers' group.

http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1221

References

Adler, A. J. (2002). Chinese religious traditions, religions of the world. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall/Laurence King.

Argue, A., Johnson, D. R., & White, L. K. (1999). Age and religiosity: Evidence from a three-wave panel analysis. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 38, 423-435.

Bond, M. H., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K.-K., & Chemonges-Nielson, Z. (2004). Combining social axioms with values in predicting social behaviours. European Journal of Personality, 18, 177-191. http://doi.org/gmj

Cao, H.-Y. (2003). An approach to the popularity of "religion tourism" in China [In Chinese]. Humanities & Social Sciences Journal of Hainan University, 21, 219-223. http://doi.org/r2w

Chaves, M. (2010). Rain dances in the dry season: Overcoming the religious congruence fallacy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 49, 1-14. http://doi.org/dtmmcz

Davie, G. (1990). Believing without belonging: Is this the future of religion in Britain? Social Compass, 37, 455-469. http://doi.org/d85kqg

Davie, G. (2006). Religion in Europe in the 21st century: The factors to take into account. European Journal of Sociology, 47, 271-296. http://doi.org/cpf8hk

Edgell, P., Gerteis, J., & Hartmann, D. (2006). Atheists as "other": Moral boundaries and cultural membership in American society. American Sociological Review, 71, 211-234. http://doi.org/dd95x2

Exline, J. J., Park, C. L., Smith, J. M., & Carey, M. P. (2011). Anger toward God: Social-cognitive predictors, prevalence, and links with adjustment to bereavement and cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 129-148. http://doi.org/d5242x

Fuller, R. C. (2001). Spiritual but not religious: Understanding unchurched America. New York: Oxford University Press.

Fung, Y. L. (1948). A short history of Chinese philosophy. London, UK: Macmillan.

Furnham, A. (1988). Lay theories: Everyday understanding of problems in the social sciences. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.

Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2012). Religiosity, social self-esteem, and psychological adjustment: On the cross-cultural specificity of the psychological benefits of religiosity. Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 23, 158-160. http://doi.org/fznb7m

Geertz, A. W., & Markusson, G. I. (2010). Religion is natural, atheism is not: On why everybody is both right and wrong. Religion, 40, 152-165. http://doi.org/dg4rww

Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science, 336, 493-496. http://doi.org/r2x

Glendinning, T., & Bruce, S. (2006). New ways of believing or belonging: Is religion giving way to spirituality? The British Journal of Sociology, 57, 399-414. http://doi.org/chvvhg

Han, X. (2003). Interpretation of religious psychology among contemporary adolescents [In Chinese]. China Religion, 7, 34-35.

Hill, P. C., Pargament, K., II, Hood, R. W. Jr., McCullough, M. E., Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., & Zinnbauer, B. J. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 30, 51-77. http://doi.org/bp3c83

Hogan, M. (2010). The culture of our thinking in relation to spirituality. New York: Nova Science.

Hua, H. (2009). A review of the phenomenon of religion among youth and undergraduates in China [In Chinese]. Theoretical Observation, 3, 129-132.

Inglehart, R., & Baker, E. W. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65, 19-51.

James, A., & Wells, A. (2002). Death beliefs, superstitious beliefs and health anxiety. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 43-53. http://doi.org/bj99fr

Johnson, D. (2012). What are atheists for? Hypotheses on the functions of non-belief in the evolution of religion. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 2, 48-70. http://doi.org/r2z

Koenig, H. G., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Religion and mental health: Evidence for an association. International Review of Psychiatry, 13, 67-78. http://doi.org/fnd6nn

Leung, K., Au, A., Huang, X., Kurman, J., Niit, T., & Niit, K.-K. (2007). Social axioms and values: A cross-cultural examination. European Journal of Personality, 21, 91-111. http://doi.org/bcn8g9

Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model of social beliefs in multicultural perspective. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 36 (pp. 119-197). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Leung, K., Bond, M. H., Reimel de Carrasquel, S., Munoz, C., Hernandez, M., Murakami, F., ... Singelis, T. M. (2002). Social axioms: The search for universal dimensions of general beliefs about how the world functions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 286-302. http://doi.org/gmx

Li, Z. G. (2006). On the oneness of heaven and man [In Chinese]. Journal of Sun Yat-sen University (Social Science Edition), 46, 49-54.

Lou, Y. L. (1994). Confucianism, Buddhism and Taoism in Chinese culture [In Chinese]. Forum on Chinese Culture, 3, 38-47.

Lou, Y. L. (2007). Awaken the "nature-ration" consciousness of cultural subjectivity [In Chinese]. Chinese Academy of Social Sciences Review, 3, 1-2.

McGrath, A. (2004). The twilight of atheism: The rise and fall of disbelief in the modern world. London, UK: Rider Books.

McGuire, M. B. (2008). Lived religion: Faith and practice in everyday life. New York: Oxford University Press.

Miller, W. R., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). Spirituality, religion, and health: An emerging research field. American Psychologist, 58, 24-35. http://doi.org/bfnvdm

Moore, R. (2005, March). We have no morality without an acknowledgment of God. Christianity Today [Website article]. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/1zPd6C2

Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012). Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal god. PLOS ONE, 7, e36880. http://doi.org/r23

Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174-187. http://doi.org/dtckwt

Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. New York: Cambridge University Press.

O'Connell, K. A., & Skevington, S. M. (2010). Spiritual, religious, and personal beliefs are important and distinctive to assessing quality of life in health: A comparison of theoretical models. British Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 729-748. http://doi.org/dtnq65

Owen, A. D., Hayward, R. D., Koenig, H. G., Steffen, D. C., & Payne, M. E. (2011). Religious factors and hippocampal atrophy in late life. PLOS ONE, 6, e17006. http://doi.org/b9br56

Pargament, K. I., Koenig, H. G., Tarakeshwar, N., & Hahn, J. (2004). Religious coping methods as predictors of psychological, physical and spiritual outcomes among medically ill elderly patients: A two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 713-730. http://doi.org/fqwbw7

Pearce, L. D., Foster, E. M., & Halliday Hardie, J. (2013). A person-centered examination of adolescent religiosity using latent class analysis. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 52, 57-79. http://doi.org/r24

Schjoedt, U., Stodkilde-Jorgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive Affect Neuroscience, 4, 199-207. http://doi.org/d7t5cb

Singelis, T. M., Hubbard, C., Her, P., & An, S. (2003). Convergent validation of the Social Axioms Survey. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 269-282. http://doi.org/gm7

Storm, I. (2009). Halfway to heaven: Four types of fuzzy fidelity in Europe. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 48, 702-718. http://doi.org/crq3j4

Tan, Z. (2007). The analysis of religious beliefs among current undergraduate students [In Chinese]. New West (theoretical version), 8, 75-75.

Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 383-388. http://doi.org/bxh5kq

Wilkinson, P. J., & Coleman, P. G. (2010). Strong beliefs and coping in old age: A case-based comparison of atheism and religious faith. Ageing & Society, 30, 337-361. http://doi.org/c75xq9 WIN-Gallup International. (2012). Global index of religiosity and atheism. bit.ly/1pw4dXG

Xu, J. L. (2013). A world miracle: Fusion of Confucianism, Buddhism, and Taoism in China [In Chinese]. People's Tribune, 12, 74-75.

Zhou, Z. F. (1985). Zhou Yi translation and annotation [In Chinese]. Beijing: Zhong Hua Press. Zuckerman, P. (2008). Society without God. New York: New York University Press.

XIAOJUAN JING

Beijing Youth Politics College and Chinese Academy of Science

Xiaojuan Jing, Department of Education and Management for Adolescents and Children, Beijing Youth Politics College; Graduate School and Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Science.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Xiaojuan Jing, 9 Huajiadi Street, Chaoyang District, Beijing 100102, People's Republic of China. Email: jingxjok@gmail.com
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and t Test for Religiosity
Scale Score and Item Score

Variable                                     Nonbelievers   Believers

                                              M      SD     M      SD

1. Religious faith contributes to good       3.00   0.86   3.22   1.35
   mental health.
2. Religion makes people escape from         2.96   0.82   2.55   1.11
   reality. *
3. Religious people are more likely          3.25   0.88   3.52   1.20
   to maintain moral standards.
4. There is a Supreme Being controlling      2.97   1.00   3.42   1.18
   the universe.
5. Religion helps us to pursue good          2.56   0.93   2.93   1.12
   fortune and avoid disaster.
6. Religious beliefs lead to unscientific    3.01   0.87   2.61   1.13
   thinking. *
7. Belief in a religion helps one            3.24   0.94   3.60   1.11
   understand the meaning of life
8. Belief in religion makes people           2.98   0.93   3.52   1.08
   good citizens
Religiosity scale score                      3.00   0.47   3.39   0.66

Variable                                          t test for N-B

                                               t       p    Cohens'
                                                               d

1. Religious faith contributes to good       -1.79    .07    -.19
   mental health.
2. Religion makes people escape from          3.69    .00     .42
   reality. *
3. Religious people are more likely          -2.21    .03    -.26
   to maintain moral standards.
4. There is a Supreme Being controlling      -3.25    .00    -.41
   the universe.
5. Religion helps us to pursue good          -2.87    .00    -.36
   fortune and avoid disaster.
6. Religious beliefs lead to unscientific    -3.28    .00    -.40
   thinking. *
7. Belief in a religion helps one            -2.83    .01    -.35
   understand the meaning of life
8. Belief in religion makes people           -4.27    .00    -.54
   good citizens
Religiosity scale score                      -5.85    .00    -.68

Variable                                          t test for N-M

                                                t       p    Cohens'
                                                                d

1. Religious faith contributes to good         -.10    .92    -.001
   mental health.
2. Religion makes people escape from          -1.16    .25    -.1
   reality. *
3. Religious people are more likely            6.90    .00     .57
   to maintain moral standards.
4. There is a Supreme Being controlling        -.66    .51    -.05
   the universe.
5. Religion helps us to pursue good          -11.49    .00    -.96
   fortune and avoid disaster.
6. Religious beliefs lead to unscientific       .24    .81    -.02
   thinking. *
7. Belief in a religion helps one              6.10    .00     .51
   understand the meaning of life
8. Belief in religion makes people             -.62    .54    -.05
   good citizens
Religiosity scale score                         .25    .81     .02

Note. * reverse-scored item, df = 577, N-B means the comparison
between nonbelievers and believers, N-M means the comparison
between nonbelievers and midpoint.
COPYRIGHT 2014 Scientific Journal Publishers, Ltd.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2014 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Jing, Xiaojuan
Publication:Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:9CHIN
Date:Aug 1, 2014
Words:4608
Previous Article:The roles of individual differences and innovation properties in multiple forms of innovation implementation.
Next Article:Obituary.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2020 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters