Printer Friendly

Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: results of a randomized controlled pilot trial.

Few social ills are as pernicious and persistent as alcohol dependence. Despite more than a century of intervention efforts, 28.4% of persons ever treated for alcohol problems remain dependent on alcohol and 19.1% continue to exhibit alcohol abuse or subclinical dependence symptoms in the past year (Dawson et al. 2005). Clearly, extant interventions are not effective for all alcohol misusers. Persons who drink to cope with stress have significantly higher rates of lifetime and current alcohol dependence symptoms than persons who drink for other reasons (Schroder & Perrine 2007), and stress is known to increase risk of relapse (Sinha 2007). Moreover, alcohol users have a comparatively high likelihood of experiencing serious life stressors: within the population of adult past-year drinkers, 72.5% reported experiencing at least one stressful life event in the past year, and 23.2% experienced three to five such stressors (Dawson, Grant & Ruan 2005). The experience of stressful life events significantly predicts quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption; for example, drinkers who reported experiencing six or more stressful events in the past year consumed, on average, more than three times the amount of ethanol per day than did alcohol users who denied experiencing any such stressors (Dawson, Grant & Ruan 2005). Congruent with such findings, an event-history analysis of 1,786 young adults found that both distal and proximal exposure to stressful life events significantly predicted onset of alcohol dependence in a linear and additive fashion after controlling for socioeconomic status and history of psychiatric disorder, implicating a causal role for life stress in the etiology of the disorder (Lloyd & Turner 2008). Convergent evidence suggests that stress is a common precipitant of alcohol misuse and may play an important role in the pathogenesis of alcohol use disorders.

The central risk chain of stress-precipitated alcohol misuse, dependence, and relapse involves cognitive-affective mechanisms that may be explicated by an integrated biopsychosocial framework (for a review, see Garland, Boettiger & Howard Under review). In brief, stress appraisals coupled with deficits in coping resources result in psychophysiological reactivity, perseverative cognition, and negative affect, which in turn trigger automatized schemata to deploy sequences of maladaptive cognitive-behavioral processes that result in compulsive alcohol consumption in spite of often severe consequences for drinking.

Stress-activated engagement of alcohol use action schemata may result in implicit (i.e., unconscious) processing of salient stimuli, manifested as an involuntary attentional bias (AB) towards alcohol cues. Such alcohol approach bias is evident in visual probe tasks, in which heavy drinkers preferentially attend to alcohol cues, resulting in decreased reaction times (RTs) to probes replacing alcohol photographs presented for 500 and 2000 milliseconds (ms) compared to probes replacing neutral photographs presented for the same duration (Field et al. 2004). Conversely, although alcohol-dependent patients have been shown to exhibit an AB toward alcohol cues presented for 50 ms (Noel et al. 2006), they evidence AB away from alcohol cues presented for 500 ms (Townshend & Duka 2007; Stormark et al. 1997). Collectively, these findings suggest that alcohol-dependent individuals in treatment, unlike their untreated counterparts, evince attentional disengagement from or avoidance of alcohol cues presented for longer stimulus durations (which allow for conscious mediation of attention), but continue to exhibit implicit appetitive attentional responses for short duration stimuli. Alcohol AB has been linked to subjective craving and alcohol consumption (Field & Eastwood 2005). Moreover, among persons who drink to cope with dysphoria, stress enhances alcohol AB and craving (Field & Powell 2007).

When their attention is fixated on visual or olfactory alcohol cues, alcohol-dependent individuals exhibit significant psychophysiological reactivity (Carter & Tiffany 1999). In turn, this alcohol cue-reactivity may lead to increased craving, which can trigger alcohol consumption as a means of reducing distress. Many persons recovering from alcohol use disorders attempt to suppress cravings, which, paradoxically, can serve to increase intrusive, automatic alcohol-related cognitions (Palfai et al. 1997), dysphoria, and autonomic arousal (Wenzlaff & Wegner 2000). Indeed, among alcohol-dependent persons, thought suppression is negatively correlated with vagally-mediated heart rate variability (Ingjaldsson, Laberg & Thayer 2003), a putative index of emotion regulation and parasympathetic inhibition of stress reactions (Thayer & Lane 2000). As thoughts of drinking intensify and are coupled with psychobiological distress, the impulse to consume alcohol as a form of palliative coping may overcome depleted self-regulation strength (Muraven & Shmueli 2006; Muraven, Collins & Nienhaus 2002) leading to relapse. The attempt to avoid distress or allay its impact through compulsive alcohol consumption results in negative reinforcement conditioning that may perpetuate this cycle by further sensitizing the brain to future stressful encounters via allostatic dysregulation of neuroendocrine systems (Koob 2003). Components of this risk chain may be especially malleable to targeted behavioral therapies.

Given that negative affect, autonomic arousal, automaticity, and attentional biases appear to be integral components of the risk chain for stress-precipitated alcohol misuse, dependence, and relapse, interventions targeting these mechanisms may hold promise for its treatment. One such intervention, mindfulness training, which originates from Buddhist traditions but has been co-opted by Western clinicians, has recently gained prominence in the psychological and medical literatures for its salutary effects on stress-related biobehavioral conditions (Greeson 2009; Ludwig & Kabat-Zinn 2008; Baer & Krietemeyer 2006). Mindfulness involves self-regulation of a metacognitive form of attention: a nonreactive, nonevaluative monitoring of moment-by-moment cognition, emotion, perception, and physiological state without fixation on thoughts of past or future (Garland 2007). A growing body of research suggests that mindfulness affects implicit cognition and attentional processes (e.g., Lutz et al. 2008; Jha, Krompinger & Baime 2007; Wenk-Sormaz 2005) as well as heart rate variability indices of parasympathetic regulation (Tang et al. 2009). This evidence, coupled with the knowledge that alcohol use disorders involve automaticity and attentional biases, suggests that mindfulness training may be an effective treatment for alcohol-dependent persons coping with stress and dysphoria.

Mindfulness treatments may enhance clinical outcomes in substance-abusing populations. Using a nonrandomized pre-post comparison group design, Bowen and colleagues (2006) found that mindfulness training of incarcerated inmates reduced post-release substance use, substance-related problems, and psychiatric symptoms to a greater extent than standard chemical dependency services offered at the prison. Other pilot studies of mindfulness-based interventions with substance abusers have found significant reductions in distress, negative affect, stress-related biomarkers, and substance use (Zgierska et al. 2008; Marcus et al. 2003; Marcus, Fine & Kouzekanani 2001). Despite this suggestive evidence that mindfulness interventions may ameliorate factors related to stress-precipitated alcohol misuse, future research should employ random assignment, as well as behavioral and psychophysiological indices of intervention-related change to overcome threats to validity and elucidate therapeutic mechanisms of action.

To that end, we conducted a pilot randomized controlled trial of a mindfulness intervention designed to disrupt cognitive, affective, and physiological risk mechanisms implicated in stress-precipitated relapse to alcohol consumption. A randomized, controlled design was used to compare the therapeutic effects of a mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement (MORE) intervention to those of an evidence-based alcohol dependence support group (ASG) in a sample of low-income, primarily African-American alcohol-dependent adults recruited from a long-term, modified therapeutic community (TC).

We hypothesized that, relative to ASG, MORE would result in significantly greater decreases in perceived stress, impaired alcohol response inhibition, craving for alcohol, psychiatric symptoms, and thought suppression and significantly greater increases in mindfulness and in heart rate variability (HRV) recovery from stress-primed alcohol cues. Lastly, we hypothesized that mindfulness training would reduce alcohol AB, a presumed mechanism of change, by the intervention midpoint and prior to changes in clinical outcomes, such that, relative to ASG, MORE would result in significantly larger decreases in alcohol AB among participants with pretreatment alcohol approach bias but not for those with baseline alcohol disengagement bias. This hypothesis was justified on the rationale that participants with pretreatment alcohol disengagement bias entered our study with a pre-existing ability to disengage their attention from alcohol cues and would therefore exhibit a floor effect.

METHOD

Sample Characteristics

Participants were alcohol-dependent adults living in a TC in the urban southeastern U.S. serving persons with substance-use disorders. The TC serves approximately 600 individuals annually, 33% of whom are homeless prior to entering the program.

Potential participants met study inclusion criteria if they were 18 years of age or older, met lifetime Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) alcohol dependence criteria, and had resided in the TC for 18 months or more. In this program, 18 months marks the time of transition to employment and residence outside of the TC, and thus represents a point of increased relapse risk. Persons with psychiatric symptoms (screened via the Brief Symptom Inventory; Derogatis & Melisaratos 1983) were included in the study, as were those with comorbid substance use disorders.

Table 1 presents sample characteristics. Study participants (N = 53) had resided in the TC for a mean of 22.3 [+ or -] 3.7 months. A majority of participants were male (79.2%), African American (60.4%), and low-income (52.8% had earned less than $20,000 in the year before entering the TC). Participants reported high rates of lifetime exposure to traumatic violence, moderate psychiatric symptomatology, and high levels of alcohol dependence severity: for example, the mean number of DSM-IV alcohol dependence criteria met by participants was 6.5 [+ or -] 1.0, and the mean number of standard alcoholic drinks consumed per day in the year before entering treatment was 19.0 [+ or -] 10.9. Approximately 81% of the sample reported daily use of at least one psychoactive drug in addition to alcohol before entering the TC, with cocaine the most frequently used drug (M = 17.1 [+ or -] 12.5 days used in the month before entering the TC). All participants reported continuous abstinence from psychoactive substance use during their residence in the TC, which ranged from 18 to 28 months. Reports of abstinence were corroborated by random urinalysis conducted at the TC, as well as through daily evaluation from program staff.

Intervention Groups

MORE. The ten-session, manualized MORE intervention was adapted as a treatment for alcohol dependence from Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy, an empirically-supported, mindfulness intervention designed to prevent depression relapse (Segal, Williams & Teasdale 2002). MORE involves mindful breathing and walking meditations, as well as experiential exercises relating general mindfulness principles to addiction-specific issues such as relapse triggers, craving, thought suppression, stress, and unconscious substance use behaviors. A Master's level social worker (MSW) with experience in mindfulness meditation who was trained in cognitive-behavioral treatments for substance dependence delivered the MORE intervention.

ASG. To control for time, attention, credibility, and group process, the ten-session ASG condition consisted of MSW-led social support groups derived from the active, evidence-based treatment condition outlined in the Matrix Model intensive outpatient treatment manual (Rawson & McCann 2006). The addictions treatment training of each MSW was comparable. Wherever possible, ASG topics were selected to roughly match corresponding themes of the MORE intervention. ASG participants were guided to disclose feelings and thoughts about group topics, as well as to provide advice and emotional support for their peers. Although the Master's-level social worker facilitated discussion using client-centered counseling skills (Rogers 2003), no prescriptions for change were given.

For fifteen minutes a day, MORE participants were asked to practice mindfulness exercises while ASG participants were asked to journal for 15 minutes per day on support group topics. A brief session by session description of the study treatment groups is detailed in Table 2.

Design and Procedure

All study procedures were approved by the University of North Carolina - Chapel Hill Institutional Review Board. Participants were recruited when they had resided at least 18 months at the TC through an informational presentation about the study made at the TC, as well as through flyers and direct referrals from TC staff. Screening procedures included administration of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor et al. 2001), the Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST; Skinner 1982) and a semistructured diagnostic interview conducted by a MSW to ensure that all participants met lifetime DSM-IV criteria for alcohol dependence. Participants were excluded if they scored less than 16 on the AUDIT, or if they endorsed screening questions indicating active psychosis (Degenhardt et al. 2005) or suicidality. Upon enrollment, participants were randomly assigned to either MORE or ASG. No participants refused randomization. Participants received $25 for completion of each assessment and $5 for attending each intervention session, with a possible bonus of $25 for perfect attendance of all assessments and sessions; total maximum: $175.

Measures

Study participants completed standardized psychosocial instruments, a psychophysiological cue-reactivity protocol, and a computer-based measurement of alcohol AB before and after 10 weeks of intervention; the alcohol AB assessment was also administered at the intervention midpoint (five weeks). Psychosocial instruments were verbally administered in interviews conducted by a research assistant who was blind to group assignment. All measures were administered in the same order across participants in a single session. Cronbach's alpha (a) is reported as an index of the internal consistency of each psychometric instrument in the present sample.

Intervention credibility. Perceived credibility of the MORE and ASG interventions as treatments for alcohol dependence was assessed after session two using a scale ([alpha] = .83) based on the Attitudes Towards Treatment measure (Borkovec & Nau 1972), which assesses patients' expectations of benefit once treatment has been explained. The scale has been found to distinguish between standard psychotherapy approaches and illogical placebo treatments, is predictive of clinical improvement, and relatively independent of symptom severity (Borkovec & Nau 1972).

Mindfulness. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ, [alpha] = .81), comprised of 39 Likert-type items, was used to measure self-reported mindfulness. The FFMQ yields a total score and scores for five internally consistent mindfulness factors each with their own convergent and predictive validity: nonreactivity to inner experience (tapped by items such as "I watch my feelings without getting lost in them"), observing and attending to experience ("I pay attention to sensations, such as the wind in my hair or the sun on my face"), describing and discriminating emotional experiences ("I'm good at finding words to describe my feelings"), nonjudging of experience ("I tell myself I shouldn't be feeling the way that I am feeling"), and acting with awareness ("I find myself doing things without paying attention") (Baer et al. 2006).

Psychosocial factors related to alcohol dependence. The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI, [alpha] = .96) was use to measure to what degree participants were currently distressed by psychiatric symptoms (Derogatis & Melisaratos 1983) on five-point scale (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely). The 53 items include symptoms such as "feeling fearful" and "feelings of guilt." Subjective alcohol craving frequency and intensity was assessed with the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale (PACS, [alpha] = .91) (Flannery et al. 2001). Self-reported ability to inhibit the urge to use alcohol was assessed with the Impaired Alcohol Response Inhibition Scale (IRISA, [alpha] = .79) (Guardia et al. 2007). Participants indicate on a four-point scale with 14 items how often over the past week they would agree with statements including "When I have decided not to drink, I find it easy not to" and "If I thought about the possibility of drinking I think I could have resisted." The 10-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10, [alpha] = .85) was used to assess on a five-point scale how often (0 = never, 4 = very often) in the past month participants found their lives unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overwhelming (Cohen, Kamarck & Mermelstein 1983), and includes items like "How often have you felt nervous and 'stressed'?" and "How often have you felt that you were on top of things?". Chronic thought suppression, the maladaptive and counterproductive tendency to avoid or repress undesirable cognitions and emotions, was assessed with the White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI, [alpha] = .84) (Wegner & Zanakos 1994). Participants indicate agreement on a five-point scale with 15 items, including "I always try to put problems out of mind" and "I often do things to distract myself from my thoughts."

Psychophysiological cue-reactivity. Due to our interest in the effects of stress on alcohol dependence, a cue-reactivity protocol was used to measure autonomic reactivity to and recovery from stress-primed alcohol cues. First, electrocardiogram (ECG) sensors were attached to participants' right and left pectoral muscles with a ground placed just above the hip. ECG was sampled at 500 Hz and recorded continuously throughout the protocol on a Biopac MP150 data acquisition system (Biopac Systems, Goleta, CA). Once electrodes were attached, participants were asked to rate their current level of distress on a 10-point visual analogue scale (VAS) (0 = not at all, 9 = extremely). Next, participants were instructed to remain motionless, silent, and "not think about anything in particular" for a five-minute baseline. After this baseline period, participants again rated their current distress to assess whether any initial reactivity elicited by the unfamiliar experimental context had stabilized after the five-minute rest period. Next, 30 aversive photographs from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) were serially presented on a 15" laptop screen for 10 seconds each (total duration: five minutes). Participants were asked to fixate on the image stream while holding as still as possible. After this presentation, participants again rated their current level of distress. Next, 30 photographs of beer, wine, and distilled liquor (12 of which included individuals drinking or preparing to drink alcohol) were serially presented for 10 seconds each (total duration: five minutes), and participants were again instructed to keep still and fixate on the image stream. At the end of the alcohol cue exposure period, participants were asked to rate their current level of distress. Lastly, participants were instructed, "Remaining silent and keeping your body still, use whatever methods you have learned to cope with or reduce your craving" for a five-minute recovery period, after which they again rated their current level of distress. A single qualitative question, "What methods did you use to cope?", identified coping strategies used.

R-R intervals were detected in the ECG data using automated routines in Nevrokard aHRV software (Medistar, Stegne, Ljubljana, Slovenia). The R-wave file was then visually inspected to correct misidentified or omitted R-waves. Kubios 2.0 (Biosignal Analysis and Medical Imaging Group, University of Finland) was used for time-domain analysis of R-R intervals. The square root of the mean squared differences between successive R-R intervals (RMSSD) was selected as our estimate of vagally mediated HRV. The present analysis focused on a particular planned contrast: HRV recovery, that is, changes in HRV between alcohol cue exposure and the recovery period.

Alcohol attentional bias. A modified dot probe task (MacLeod, Mathews & Tata 1986) created in E-Prime 1.2 (PST Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) and presented on an IBM T60 laptop with a 15" screen was used to measure alcohol AB (for methodological details, see Garland et al. Under review). In brief, for each trial, two grayscale images appeared, one on each side of a fixation cross (+); one image was neutral in content, the other was alcohol-related. Each pair of images was presented for either 200 ms or 2000 ms. Left/right position of the alcohol images and presentation duration were both randomized and counterbalanced across 20 practice trials and 160 trials. Following a 50 ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI), a target (two dots) replaced one of the images and a distracter probe (one dot) replaced the other image; probes appeared for 100 ms. Participants were instructed to fixate on the cross between the images and indicate the location of the target by responding with a left or right button press on a keypad. Target probes pseudorandomly replaced alcohol and neutral images with equal frequency.

Alcohol stimuli included 13 photographs of alcoholic drinks, and seven photos of persons drinking alcohol. Neutral stimuli included 13 photos of kitchen items and seven photos of persons in kitchen scenes. Stimulus sets were analyzed with respect to their spatial frequency content to ensure that they did not differ in terms of basic visual properties, which could elicit reflexive attentional capture irrespective of image content. On measures of both the spectral peak (Neutral: 0.0180, Alcohol: 0.0176, t(38)=0.383, p = 0.704) and spectral width (Neutral: 59.20, Alcohol: 59.29, t(38=0.027, p = 0.979), the two stimulus sets were not significantly different.

Data Analysis

Data were examined for outliers and to ensure they met distributional assumptions for normality. All outcome variables were approximately normally distributed, save for heart rate variability data (RMSSD) which was skewed and therefore log-transformed (log^). Bivariate correlations, t-tests, chi-square tests, and repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) as implemented in SPSS 17.0 were used to test hypotheses, compare group differences at baseline and over time, and explore individual differences in the change process. When necessary, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were used to address violations of sphericity.

For AB data, trials with extreme RTs (more than 3 SD above the individual mean) were discarded as outliers (mean = 2.5 [+ or -] 1.5 per participant); error trials were also discarded (mean = 4.9 [+ or -].08) . For each participant, AB scores were calculated by subtracting their mean RT to probes replacing alcohol photos from their mean RT to probes replacing neutral photos, such that positive bias scores indicate an AB toward visual alcohol cues.

RESULTS

Pre-intervention Group Differences

There were no significant pre-intervention differences between MORE and ASG participants on age, gender, race, income, number of months residing in the TC, alcohol dependence criteria, AUDIT, DAST, exposure to traumatic violence, craving, impaired alcohol response inhibition, psychiatric symptoms, mindfulness, thought suppression, or alcohol attentional bias (Table 1). Similarly, there were no significant differences between MORE and ASG participants in HRV responses during the pre-intervention cue-reactivity protocol. The only difference approaching statistical significance (p = .094) was for number of drinks/day, such that participants randomized to MORE had marginally higher levels of alcohol consumption before entering the TC than those randomized to ASG. Similarly, there were no significant differences between intervention completers and drop outs on any of the variables listed above. In addition, there was no significant difference between MORE and ASG participants in intervention credibility ratings on the modified Borkovec and Nau (1972) Attitudes Towards Treatment measure (mean = 39.4 [+ or -] 6.4, and 40.6 [+ or -] 4.7, respectively; F(1, 41) = .497, p = .485).

Intervention Compliance and Attrition

Approximately 69% (N = 37) of the total enrolled sample (N = 53) completed post-intervention assessments, with 18 participants completing the MORE intervention and 19 completing the ASG intervention; attrition did not significantly differ between groups. Similarly, the groups did not differ in attendance; MORE participants completed 8.0 [+ or -] 2.1 sessions, whereas ASG participants completed 7.3 [+ or -] 3.5 sessions.

Outcomes

Table 3 summarizes changes in clinical and attentional variables over the course of both the MORE and ASG interventions.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Intervention effects on self-report measures. Both MORE and ASG led to significant reductions in perceived stress over time, F(1, 35) = 18.11, p < .001, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .34. Moreover, there was a significant intervention x time interaction on perceived stress, F(1, 35) = 5.06, p = .03, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .13, such that MORE led to significantly larger decreases in perceived stress over a 10-week period than did ASG (Figure 1). Although there were no main effects of time on thought suppression, there was a significant intervention x time interaction on thought suppression, F(1, 35) = 4.26, p = .04, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .11, such that MORE led to significant decreases in thought suppression over a 10-week period, whereas an ASG led to increased thought suppression (Figure 2). Conversely, while there was a significant decrease in psychiatric symptoms over time, F(1, 35) = 10.83, p = .002, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .24, there was no significant intervention x time interaction on psychiatric symptoms. With regard to self-reported mindfulness, there were neither main effects nor an intervention x time interaction effect on changes in FFMQ scores, including subscales. Similarly, there was an absence of both main and interaction effects on craving and impaired alcohol response inhibition, suggesting that both interventions had negligible effects on alcohol urges and neither reduced craving more than the other.

Intervention effects on alcohol attentional bias. Despite the fact that pre-intervention 2000 and 200 ms AB data were both approximately normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests p's = .97 and .26, respectively), approximately one-half of participants exhibited 2000 and 200 ms AB towards probes replacing alcohol photos while the other half exhibited AB towards probes replacing neutral photos. Given that AB towards alcohol cues is theorized to represent appetitive conditioning (Field & Cox 2008) while AB away from alcohol cues is thought to represent disengagement from or avoidance of the substance (Townshend & Duka 2007), we analyzed data from participants with these two apparently distinct types of AB separately. Thus, for each presentation duration (2000 ms and 200 ms) we divided participants into two groups: those with an AB > 0, i.e., the 200 ms (N = 18) and 2000 ms (N = 16) alcohol approach groups, and those with an AB < 0, i.e., the 200 ms (N = 19) and 2000 ms (N = 21) alcohol disengagement groups.

Among the 200ms alcohol approach bias group, there was a significant intervention x time interaction on 200 ms AB, F(2, 32) = 4.76, p = .03, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .23 (Figure 3). Within-subjects contrasts revealed a significant quadratic form to the intervention x time interaction, F(1, 16) = 8.09, p = .01. For the 2000ms alcohol approach bias group, 2000 ms AB significantly decreased over the course of both interventions, F(2, 12) = 4.54, p = .02, and there was no significant intervention x time effect.

Intervention effects on heart rate variability responses to stress-primed alcohol cues. Due to hardware problems during physiological recording, complete post-intervention HRV data was available for 18 MORE and 16 ASG participants. To examine the effects of the intervention on HRV responses to alcohol cue-exposure following experimental stress-induction, we controlled for severity of alcohol dependence by including the number of drinks consumed a day prior to entering the TC as a covariate. Because tonic levels of perceived stress are known influence HRV, we also covaried post-intervention perceived stress to examine intervention effects on HRV responses to the experimental alcohol cue-reactivity paradigm. Although the overall F-test for the intervention x experimental condition (baseline, stress exposure, alcohol exposure, recovery) interaction was nonsignificant, there was a significant intervention x condition interaction on the planned contrast between alcohol cue exposure and the recovery period, F(1,30) = 5.30, p = .03, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .15, indicating that the pattern of HRV from alcohol cue exposure to recovery varied as a function of intervention. MORE participants had higher HRV during alcohol cue exposure which then decreased during the recovery period, whereas ASG participants had lower HRV during alcohol cue exposure which then increased during the recovery period (see Figure 4). No such significant intervention x condition effect on HRV was found during the baseline assessment after covarying for severity of alcohol dependence and pre-intervention perceived stress, lending support to the interpretation that the observed post-intervention between-groups differences in HRV recovery were the result of the MORE and ASG interventions.

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]

[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]

Qualitative inquiry indicated that during the postintervention cue-reactivity protocol, all MORE participants practiced mindfulness meditation during the recovery period while the most common strategies used by ASG participants included cognitive reappraisal (e.g., focus on the negative consequences of drinking and the positive benefits of sobriety) as well as suppression (e.g., tried to think about something else). With regard to subjective distress during the cue-reactivity protocol, after controlling for prior level of alcohol consumption and tonic post-intervention perceived stress, MORE participants had significantly larger reductions in distress from alcohol cue-exposure to the recovery period than did ASG participants, F(1,32) = 4.94, p = .03, [eta][[??].sup.2] = .13.

Individual difference variables and changes in stress and addiction-related factors. To determine whether improvements in self-reported ability to inhibit alcohol responses were associated with individual differences in baseline stress, changes in impaired alcohol response inhibition (as measured by the IRISA) were correlated with pre-intervention perceived stress. Among MORE participants, the greatest improvements in self-reported ability to inhibit alcohol responses were found among those with highest pre-intervention levels of perceived stress, r = .52, p = .03. Among ASG participants, these variables were not significantly correlated, indicating that the relationship between pre-treatment stress and improved ability to inhibit alcohol responses may be attributable to the MORE intervention. Thus, it appears that MORE is most effective at inhibiting the appetitive alcohol responses of persons who experience high levels of stress at baseline, i.e. persons who may be more likely to drink as a means of coping with stress.

Importantly, among MORE participants, changes in thought suppression were correlated with changes in AB200 (r = .49, p = .042), changes in impaired alcohol response inhibition (r = .48, p = .045), and changes in post-intervention HRV recovery, (r = .49, p = .045). MORE participants who experienced greater decreases in thought suppression over the course of intervention also evidenced greater decreases in AB200 and impaired alcohol response inhibition while evincing the greater HRV recovery. However, among ASG participants, there was no significant correlation between these variables. In contrast, across the entire sample of persons who completed either intervention, decreases in stress were positively correlated with decreases in impaired alcohol response inhibition (r = .33, p = .046).

DISCUSSION

The present results suggest that mindfulness training may affect cognitive, affective, and physiological risk mechanisms implicated in relapse to alcohol dependence. Mindfulness training appeared to reduce stress and alcohol thought suppression to a significantly greater extent than did an evidence-based active control intervention. Notably, mindfulness training seems to decrease AB towards brief (200ms) visual alcohol cues, an implicit cognitive process linked with alcohol dependence, and increase HRV recovery from alcohol cues following stress induction. In addition, MORE appears to be a feasible intervention to prevent stress-precipitated alcohol relapse, based on the high perceived treatment credibility and program adherence, and the absence of significant differences in attrition from an ASG, the current standard of care.

Mindfulness Training Reduces Stress and Thought Suppression

Among recovering alcohol-dependent individuals, mindfulness training appears to be a potentially effective stress reduction technique. MORE reduced perceived stress to a greater extent than did ASG, which is noteworthy given that social support reduces stress reactivity and buffers deleterious effects of stressful life events (Christenfeld & Gerin 2000). The stress reduction effects of mindfulness training among nonclinical populations are well known in the literature (Grossman et al. 2004), but it is notable that significant effects were obtained in a sample of clinically-disordered, alcohol-dependent adults with extensive trauma histories who may be more vulnerable to stress-precipitated relapse due to allostatic dysregulation of neural stress circuitry (Valdez & Koob 2004).

Like stress, thought suppression significantly decreased over the course of ten weeks of mindfulness training. In turn, decreases in thought suppression among MORE participants were significantly correlated with decreases in impaired alcohol response inhibition, raising the possibility that participants who improved their ability to regulate drinking urges may have done so via reductions in thought suppression. Alternatively, MORE participants who experienced attenuated drinking urges may have had less need to engage in thought suppression. Our finding that mindfulness training reduced thought suppression partially replicates results reported by Bowen and colleagues (2007) within the context of our randomized controlled design, although limitations of statistical power in the present study preclude replication of their tests for mediation. Thought suppression appears to play a substantial role in psychopathology (Purdon 1999), by increasing the rate, intensity, and accessibility of the very thoughts and moods it is directed against (Wegner et al. 1987), as well as intensifying sympathetic nervous system activity (Gross 2002; Wenzlaff & Wegner 2000; Gross & Levenson 1993). In the context of alcohol dependence, thought suppression seems to enhance the conscious awareness of alcohol-related cognitions and affective reactions. MORE, with its emphasis on nonjudgmental, metacognitive awareness of present-moment experience, appeared to counter this deleterious cognitive strategy and therefore may have prevented post-suppression rebound effects from exacerbating negative affect and intrusive alcohol-related cognitions that can promote relapse. Conversely, participants in the alcohol-dependence support group may have professed invulnerability to alcohol urges for the purposes of social conformity and maintaining an appearance of competence, thereby leading to the increased thought suppression observed in the ASG data.

Contrary to our hypotheses, MORE did not significantly increase self-reported mindfulness, nor did it result in significant decrements in craving. This counterintuitive finding may be explained by the nature of the participants and by the mindfulness training itself. Prior to mindfulness training, the alcohol-dependent individuals sampled in this study may have overestimated their level of mindfulness due to their lack of understanding of the construct and their general lack of self-awareness, which was one of the intended targets of mindfulness training itself. After ten weeks of mindfulness practice reflecting on their own internal experience, MORE participants may have had a greater sense of the meaning of the FFMQ questions, and having encountered numerous challenges in their attempts to embody the principles of nonreactivity, nonjudgment, acting with awareness, etc., may have been able to more accurately appraise their level of mindfulness. Similarly, the alcohol-dependent study participants residing in a therapeutic community where access and exposure to alcohol was limited may have lacked awareness of the extent to which they continued to have drinking urges, leading to an underreporting of craving at baseline. This lack of awareness of alcohol reactivity that has been shown to be predictive of future relapse (Rohsenow et al. 1994). Because MORE was partly designed to increase consciousness of craving and decrease urge suppression, ten weeks of the intervention may have heightened awareness of latent alcohol urges, thus resulting in an apparent lack of change in craving over time. Of course, such interpretations are speculative, and a number of alternative interpretations are possible, such as inaccurate reporting due to social desirability bias, ceiling/floor effects due to previous participation in a therapeutic community, and the possibility of true null effects of MORE on mindfulness and craving.

In contrast to the null effects on craving, both interventions led to statistically significant reductions in psychiatric symptoms. The fact that ASG exerted a treatment effect comparable to that of MORE indicates that the control condition used in this study was therapeutically active. Moreover, this finding confirms those of Bowen and colleagues (2006) that mindfulness training decreases psychiatric symptoms among substance abusers. However, given that no significant differences were found between intervention conditions, it cannot be ruled out that the reductions in psychiatric symptoms observed were due to placebo effects, the passage of time, or other elements of the TC.

Mindfulness Training Augments Psychophysiological Recovery from Alcohol Cues

MORE and ASG had differential effects on HRV during a stress-primed alcohol cue-reactivity paradigm. Although there were no significant differences at baseline, after 10 weeks of intervention, MORE participants, relative to ASG participants, exhibited high HRV during alcohol cue exposure, which then decreased as they practiced mindfulness meditation during the recovery period. Given that greater HRV has been linked to greater prefrontal inhibition of stress-induced sympathetic nervous system activation during emotion regulation tasks (Thayer & Lane 2009), the pattern observed among MORE participants may be interpreted as evidence of engagement of emotion regulation processes during alcohol cue exposure followed by disengagement of cognitive resources from alcohol-related cognitions and cravings during the recovery period. This interpretation is consistent with conceptualizations of mindfulness as involving a "letting go" of distressing mental content (Baer & Krietemeyer 2006). However, lower HRV may also be related to the rhythmic pacing of the breath that may stem from novice meditation techniques of breath focus and counting (Peressutti et al. 2009). These techniques may work as a rhythmic stimulus that entrains the breath and reduces overall HRV. Alternatively, lower HRV during recovery may indicate reduced parasympathetic efference associated with focused, concentrated attention on the breath. While highly experienced meditators may show increased HRV during mindfulness practice, novice meditators, for whom meditation on the breath is cognitively demanding, exhibit decreased HRV during meditation practice (Peressutti et al. 2009). Heightened cognitive load has been shown to suppress HRV during working memory and sustained attention tasks (Hansen, Johnsen & Thayer 2003). Thayer and Lane (2000: 207) assert that "appropriate short-term suppression of HRV" during attentional engagement is critical for self-regulation.

Other converging evidence supports these interpretations. Indeed, the neurovisceral integration model (Thayer & Lane 2000) posits that as central autonomic network activity increases, HRV increases. Thus, HRV should increase proportionally to the intensity of conscious processing of emotion and emotion regulation. Indeed, relative to ASG participants, MORE participants had higher post-intervention HRV during stress and alcohol cue exposure (despite having lower pre-intervention levels of HRV), perhaps indicating the marshalling of greater cognitive control in service of regulating negative emotions triggered by aversive images and cravings elicited by alcohol cues. Such HRV effects have been found to result from the control of urges: high self-regulation efforts to resist temptation result in increased HRV (Segerstrom & Nes 2007), and alcoholics who report a greater ability to regulate alcohol urges experience increased HRV when exposed to alcohol cues relative to those who report less ability to regulate alcohol-use compulsions (Ingjaldsson, Laberg & Thayer 2003).

In contrast, the post-intervention ASG group evidenced phasic suppression of HRV during stress and alcohol cue exposure, a pattern that seems to indicate disruption of homeostasis, anxiety, and stress reactivity (Thayer & Lane 2000). Subsequently, ASG participants increased HRV during the recovery period when they were engaged in emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal and suppression. These strategies may have required a high level of PFC activation, thus leading to the increased HRV observed among ASG participants during the recovery period, an interpretation supported by research showing increased HRV during reappraisal and suppression (Butler, Wilhelm & Gross 2006).

Hypothetically, this pattern of cardiac response may indicate the superior efficiency of mindfulness as an emotion regulation strategy relative to reappraisal, distraction, and suppression strategies, an inference supported by our self-report data that show that MORE led to greater reductions in subjective distress from alcohol cue-exposure to recovery. Concomitantly, for those participants trained in mindfulness, larger pre-post reductions in thought suppression were correlated with greater post-intervention HRV recovery from alcohol cue exposure. We speculate that those MORE participants who most successfully learned to employ mindfulness to cope with alcohol-related cognitions and craving instead of suppression were able to disengage from those mental phenomena more efficiently, as reflected by increased HRV recovery. In contrast, alcohol-dependent persons who continued to use thought suppression as a strategy may have remained fixated on alcohol-related cognitions and cues due to the post-suppression rebound effect, resulting in less HRV recovery which may be reflective of continued ruminative engagement (Key et al. 2008).

Mindfulness Training Modulates Implicit Alcohol Attentional Bias

Intervention effects on 200 ms alcohol AB may also index degree of disengagement from alcohol-related cognitions. Given that mindfulness training has been shown to impact attentional orienting and alerting processes (Jha, Krompinger & Baime 2007), we hypothesized that MORE would impact 200ms AB in persons with alcohol approach biases at baseline. Although this hypothesis was supported by the data, we expected a simple decreasing linear change trajectory and not the apparently u-shaped, quadratic pattern observed. What could account for the pattern of results seen in the AB data? During the first five weeks of the MORE intervention, participants were encouraged to decenter from their cognitive-emotional experience and become aware of whatever thoughts and feelings arose during the meditation sessions. Several mindfulness exercises involved imaginal alcohol exposure in order to teach methods to deal mindfully with craving. During these exercises, participants were encouraged to imagine having strong alcohol cravings and to notice the resultant physiological and affective reactivity. In addition, participants were asked to log daily experiences of craving and to carefully attend to the psychophysiological concomitants of the craving experience. Participation in these exercises may have activated alcohol use action schema, triggering implicit positive alcohol expectancies through the imagination of conditioned, hedonic stimuli, resulting in the deployment of a conditioned attentional response, i.e. increases in the 200 ms AB. As the mindfulness participants became more proficient at self-regulating attention and developing metacognitive awareness of automatic response patterns, this effect may have been attenuated by the post-intervention time point.

Conversely, by five weeks into the alcohol support group intervention, participants may have felt considerable pressure to conform to the culture of the group, which emphasized an abstinence-only attitude toward recovery from alcohol dependence. Group culture focused on the negative consequences of drinking, and the climate of the group was such that verbal pronouncements of one's ability to resist the temptation to drink were reinforced via social influence, social norms, and/or social control. These discussions may have activated negative implicit alcohol expectancies and therefore attenuated the 200 ms AB. However, post-intervention, when the social support group was officially disbanded at the end of the 10-week intervention, the influence and norms afforded by the group culture were no longer in place. Without this social control element, appetitive tendencies towards alcohol may have been unchecked, and given the lack of skill acquisition in the support group, participants may have had no means of coping with implicit cognitive biases towards alcohol.

To determine the clinical significance of the small yet statistically significant change in 200 ms AB over time among MORE participants, post-intervention craving was regressed onto pre-post change in 200 ms AB for MORE participants with alcohol approach biases (N = 9). Pre-post change in 200 ms AB significantly predicted postintervention craving, [beta] = .79, p = .01, R-squared = .62. The finding that decreases in 200 ms AB over the course of mindfulness intervention predicted higher craving at the end of the intervention is counterintuitive. This seemingly paradoxical finding may be understood in light of Tiffany's (1990) Cognitive Processing Model of Addiction, which posits that conscious craving results from inhibition of automatized alcohol use action schema. The 200 ms AB has been considered an automatic, unconscious bias in initial attentional orienting towards alcohol-related stimuli (Field & Cox 2008). Stimuli presented for 200 ms are too fast for conscious deployment of attention; therefore, initial orienting to such stimuli may be driven by implicit appetitive processes that can detect and operate on nonconscious stimuli. According to Tiffany's theory, when alcohol use action schemas operate in an unobstructed fashion, there is an absence of craving; however, interruption of automatic, appetitive cognitive-behavioral responses promotes craving.

Training in mindfulness meditation may lead to disruption of maladaptive, automatized cognitive-behavioral reactions. Deikman (1966) conceptualized the effect of meditation as a form of "deautomatization," that is, an undoing of automatization whereby unconscious, habitual patterns of perception and motor behavior are reinvested with attention. It is plausible that during the course of the MORE intervention, automatized alcohol use action schema were deautomatized through formal and informal mindfulness practices, which involve the intentional and conscious direction of attention to cognitions, emotions, physiology, and behavior. Given that such conscious attentional processing disrupts automatic processing (Lieberman 2003), mindfulness training may have interrupted the alcohol use action schema, which Tiffany's (1990) theory would predict to result in an increase in conscious craving. This prediction appears to have been borne out in the present data linking decreased automatic alcohol attentional bias to increased craving. This finding also is sensible in the context of the findings of Rohsenow and colleagues (1994), who found that that greater awareness of physiological reactivity during alcohol cue exposure predicted drinking among alcohol-dependent men three months post-intervention while urge to drink was not a significant predictor of future drinking behavior. Increased awareness of craving-related reactivity may indicate that automatic addictive patterns have been disrupted, which may allow for the strategic deployment of self-regulatory mechanisms, eventuating in decreased drinking over time.

The identified effects of MORE on thought suppression also support a deautomatization interpretation of the correlation between mindfulness-induced reductions in 200 ms AB and higher post-intervention craving, and notably, among MORE participants, reductions in thought suppression were significantly correlated with decreases in 200 ms AB. Over time, mindfulness training may reduce the tendency to suppress one's thoughts and feelings, thereby allowing cravings that had been previously suppressed to surface to consciousness. In so doing, automatic appetitive schema (indexed by 200 ms AB) which may have been operating unchecked by controlled cognitive processing during active suppression may become accessible to explicit cognitive control. Thus, as thought suppression decreases, controlled cognitive processing can be more effectively deployed to inhibit and counter appetitive responding (as evidenced by the significant correlation between decreases in thought suppression and decreases in impaired alcohol response inhibition), resulting in decreased attentional capture by alcohol cues coupled with the conscious experience of craving as alcohol use action schema are disrupted. Through mindfulness, alcohol urges can be attended to in a nonreactive, nonjudgmental fashion, obviating the need for suppression. In so doing, cravings may extinguish over time, instead of intensifying as a result of suppression attempts.

Alternatively, the counterintuitive relationship between decreased 200 ms AB and increased craving may be explained by the methodological issues that plague self-report measures of craving, such as lack of consensus on the operationalization of the construct, the temporal scope of the craving measurement, and inconsistency between psychological and physiological indices of craving (Monti, Rohsenow & Hutchison 2000; Tiffany, Carter & Singleton 2000). If awareness of craving increased after ten weeks of mindfulness training, differences in pre-post craving scores may be biased, leading to spurious correlations between change in craving and 200 ms AB. In contrast, performance on a dot probe task with alcohol stimuli presented for 200 ms latencies may be a more valid indicator of appetitive tendencies towards alcohol than self-reports of craving.

Limitations

The primary limitation of this study is its small sample size, which limits statistical power and generalizability of the findings. Generalizability of study findings may also be limited by the sample selection and the absence of random sampling--participants who had resided for at least 18 months in a TC self-selected into the study, and thus had been exposed to a therapeutic milieu for an unusually long length of time relative to a 28-day residential or intensive outpatient treatment format. Due to the probable high motivation and competence of participants who had successfully maintained sobriety for over 18 months while residing in the TC, MORE and ASG may have had greater salutary effects in this sample than in persons with less commitment or ability to change. Conversely, because persons who could not maintain sobriety for a substantial length of time (i.e., those who were most severely impaired or dependent) were not included in this study, intervention effects may have been underestimated for individuals who might have shown the greatest benefit from MORE or ASG. Another substantial limitation was that self-report measures were administered during face-to-face interviews, which may have led to social desirability bias in self-reported outcomes. Our interpretations of HRV data may be confounded because we were unable to control for the effects of respiration (Grossman & Taylor 2007), although there is ample debate in the literature regarding the need for such corrections (e.g., Denver, Reed & Porges 2007). Given the presence of counterintuitive findings, this study needs to be replicated with a larger and more broadly representative sample, and additional research is needed to explore the clinical significance of the effect of mindfulness training on stress, thought suppression, HRV, and alcohol attentional bias. To that end, we are collecting follow-up data and will examine how intervention-related changes in these cognitive, affective, and physiological variables predict alcohol relapse.

By a three-month post-intervention follow-up, few participants from either treatment group had relapsed. However, anecdotal reports suggested that mindfulness training was perceived to be useful for preventing stress-precipitated relapse. For example, one MORE participant who was transitioning out of the TC and had obtained employment at a restaurant that served alcohol reported that her practice of mindfulness helped her to cope with cravings when stressed on the job. During a follow-up interview, this participant stated:
   I had to practice it [mindfulness] every day, when I went out
   looking for a job, and being rejected, because of my not working in
   two years, using the techniques that were being taught to me, in my
   everyday life. Yeah, I use it every day . . . I work at Red
   Lobster, where they do serve alcohol, and it [mindfulness] played a
   very big part when I had to deal with some customers that were
   drinking. And, it played a big part in helping me get through it. I
   had to go outside and breathe. Well, by me using it every day,
   breathing, taking time to sit down and think about what I'm going
   to do before I react, not reacting on impulse. I was very
   self-destructive, and if something got in my way, I would hide
   behind the alcohol and the drugs to get past it, and so now instead
   of doing that, I take whatever time I need to sit down and collect
   my thoughts, and not stay in my head about nothing.


Although event-history analyses are needed to determine whether MORE affects the rate and timing of relapse, such clinical anecdotes indicate that mindfulness training may reduce the occurrence of relapse in high-risk situations.

Conclusion

In sum, the unwitting attempts of recovering alcohol-dependent persons to suppress appetitive cognitive-emotional reactions towards alcohol may obscure these responses from consciousness only to perpetuate and intensify them within the cognitive unconscious. In the domain of unconscious mental life, automatic processes run smoothly and efficiently uninhibited by volitional control (Kihlstrom 1987). Hence, by shunting appetitive reactions into the unconscious, the alcohol-dependent individual may increase the very appetitive response towards alcohol he or she is trying to suppress and exacerbate psychophysiological reactivity to alcohol cues. Mindfulness training may serve to undo this process, making unconscious responses conscious. Thus, practice of mindfulness may promote the recovery of alcohol-dependent persons through: (a) deautomatization of alcohol use action schema, resulting in diminished attentional bias towards subliminal alcohol cues and increased craving as a result of disrupted automaticity; and (b) decreased thought suppression resulting in increased awareness of alcohol urges over time, increased HRV recovery from alcohol cue-exposure, and improved ability to inhibit appetitive responses.

Accordingly, mindfulness training may be a tractable means of promoting enduring behavior change. Although brief motivational interventions may be highly effective at impelling the desire towards sobriety, participants of such motivational enhancement therapies remain prone to eventual relapse; indeed, relapse is often a part of the recovery process. As such, interventions that consolidate short-term treatment gains into broader lifestyle change are of major significance to the addictions treatment field. During the gradual practice of mindfulness, one learns to work with negative emotions in a metacognitive context, resulting in nonreactivity to difficult mental contents and improved self-regulation in the face of stressors (Garland et al. In press). The developmental process of cultivating and embedding mindfulness principles into all aspects of one's life may solidify gains made in prior treatment and provide an effective, long-term approach to coping with stress-precipitated relapse.

Despite evidence suggesting that stress appraisal and attentional biases are key components of alcohol dependence, the form of addictions treatment most available to poor and marginalized persons, social support groups, does not target these pathogenic mechanisms directly. In contrast, practice of mindfulness may attenuate stress reactivity and thought suppression while disrupting addictive automaticity, resulting in increased awareness of craving and greater ability to cope with and recover from alcohol urges in stressful contexts. Hence, mindfulness training may hold promise as an alternative, targeted treatment for stress-precipitated alcohol dependence among vulnerable members of society.

REFERENCES

Baer, R.A. & Krietemeyer, J. 2006. Overview of mindfulness- and acceptance-based treatment approaches. In: R. A. Baer (Ed.) Mindfulness-Based Treatment Approaches: Clinician's Guide to Evidence Base and Applications. Boston, MA: Elsevier.

Baer, R.A.; Smith, G.T.; Hopkins, J.; Krietemeyer, J. & Toney, L. 2006. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13 (1): 27-45.

Babor, T.F.; Higgins-Biddle, C.; Saunders, J.B. & Monteiro, M.G. 2001. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Second Ed. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Borkovec, T. & Nau, S. 1972. Credibility of analogue therapy rationales. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 3 (4): 257-60.

Bowen, S.; Witkiewitz, K.; Dillworth, T.M. & Marlatt, G.A. 2007. The role of thought suppression in the relationship between mindfulness meditation and alcohol use. Addictive Behavior 32: 2324-28.

Bowen, S; Witkiewitz, K.; Dillworth, T.M.; Chawla, N.; Simpson, T.L.; Ostafin, B.; Larimer, M.E., Blume, A.W.; Parks, G.A. & Marlatt, G.A. 2006. Mindfulness meditation and substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 20: 343-47.

Butler, E.A. ; Wilhelm, F.H. & Gross, J.J. 2006. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Psychophysiology 43 (6): 612-22.

Carter, B.L. & Tiffany, S.T. 1999. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction 94 (3): 327-40.

Christenfeld, N. & Gerin, W. 2000. Social support and cardiovascular reactivity. Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy 54 (5): 251-57.

Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. 1983. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health, Society, and Behavior 24 (4): 385-96.

Dawson, D.A.; Grant, B.F. & Ruan, W.J. 2005. The association between stress and drinking: Modifying effects of gender and vulnerability. Alcohol & Alcoholism 40 (5): 453-60.

Dawson, D.A.; Grant, B.F.; Stinson, F.S.; Chou, P.S.; Huang, B. & Ruan, W.J. 2005. Recovery from DSM-IV alcohol dependence: United States, 2001-2002. Addiction 100 (3): 281-92.

Degenhardt, L.; Hall, W.; Korten, A. & Jablensky, A. 2005. Use of a Brief Screening Instrument for Psychosis: Results of an ROC Analysis. NDARC Technical Report No. 210. New South Wales, Australia: National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre.

Deikman, A.J. 1966. Deautomatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry 29: 324-88.

Denver, J.W.; Reed, S.F. & Porges, S.W. 2007. Methodological issues in the quantification of respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Biological Psychology 74 (2): 286-94.

Derogatis, L.R. & Melisaratos, N. 1983. The Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine 13 (3): 595-605.

Field, M. & Cox, W.M. 2008. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 97 (1-2): 1-20.

Field, M. & Powell, H. 2007. Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol and Alcoholism 42 (6): 560-66.

Field, M. & Eastwood, B. 2005. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 183 (3): 350-57.

Field, M.; Mogg, K.; Zetteler, J. & Bradley, B.P. 2004. Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: The roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 176 (1): 88-93.

Flannery, B.A.; Roberts, A.J.; Cooney, N.; Swift, R.M.; Anton, R.F. & Rohsenow, D.J. 2001. The role of craving in alcohol use, dependence, and treatment. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25 (2): 299-308.

Garland, E.L. 2007. The meaning of mindfulness: A second-order cybernetics of stress, metacognition, and coping. Complementary Health Practice Review 12 (1): 15-30.

Garland, E.L.; Boettiger, C.A. & Howard, M.O. Under review. Targeting cognitive-affective risk mechanisms in stress-precipitated alcohol dependence: An integrated, biopsychosocial model.

Garland, E.L.; Boettiger, C.A.; Gaylord, S.A.; West Chanon, V.M. & Howard, M.O. Under review. Mindfulness is inversely associated with alcohol attentional bias among recovering alcohol-dependent adults.

Garland, E.L.; Fredrickson, B.L.; Kring, A.M.; Johnson, D.P.; Meyer, P.S. & Penn, D.L. In press. Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review.

Greeson, J.M. 2009. Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health Practice Review 14 (1): 10-18.

Gross, J.J. 2002. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39 (3): 281-91.

Gross, J.J. & Levenson, R.W. 1993. Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64 (6): 970-86.

Grossman, P. & Taylor, E. W. 2007. Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology 74 (2): 263-85.

Grossman, P.; Niemann, L.; Schmidt, S. & Walach, H. 2004. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 57 (1): 35-43.

Guardia, J.; Trujols, J.; Burguete, T.; Luquero, E. & Cardus, M. 2007. Impaired response inhibition scale for alcoholism (IRISA): Development and psychometric properties of a new scale for abstinence-oriented treatment of alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 31 (2): 269-75.

Hansen, A.L.; Johnsen, B.H. & Thayer, J.F. 2003. Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology 48 (3): 263-74.

Ingjaldsson, J.T.; Laberg, J.C. & Thayer, J.F. 2003. Reduced heart rate variability in chronic alcohol abuse: Relationship with negative mood, chronic thought suppression, and compulsive drinking. Biological Psychiatry 54 (12): 1427-36.

Jha, A.; Krompinger, J. & Baime, M. 2007. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 7 (2): 109-19.

Key, B.L.; Campbell, T.S.; Bacon, S.L. & Gerin, W. 2008. The influence of trait and state rumination on cardiovascular recovery from a negative emotional stressor. Journal of Behavior Medicine 31 (3): 237-48.

Kihlstrom, J.F. 1987. The cognitive unconscious. Science 237 (4821): 1445-52.

Koob, G.F. 2003. Alcoholism: Allostasis and beyond. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27 (2): 232-43.

Lieberman, M.D. 2003. Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In: J.P. Forgas; K. Williams & W.V. Hippel (Eds.) Social Judgments: Implicit and Explicit Processes. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lloyd, D.A. & Turner, R.J. 2008. Cumulative lifetime adversities and alcohol dependence in adolescence and young adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (3): 217-26.

Ludwig, D.S. & Kabat-Zinn, J. 2008. Mindfulness in medicine. Journal of the American Medical Association 300 (11): 1350-52.

Lutz, A.; Slagter, H.A.; Dunne, J.D. & Davidson, R.J. 2008. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (4): 163-69.

MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A. & Tata, P 1986. Attentional biases in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95: 15-20.

Marcus, M.T.; Fine, M. & Kouzekanani, K. 2001. Mindfulness-based meditation in a therapeutic community. Journal of Substance Use 5: 303-11.

Marcus, M.T.; Fine, M.; Moeller, G.; Khan, M.M.; Pitts, K.; Swank, P.R. & Liehr, P. 2003. Change in stress levels following mindfulness-based stress reduction in a therapeutic community. Addictive Disorders and their Treatment 2: 63-68.

Monti, P.M.; Rohsenow, D.J. & Hutchison, K.E. 2000. Toward bridging the gap between biological, psychobiological and psychosocial models of alcohol craving. Addiction 95 (Supplement 2): S229-36.

Muraven, M. & Shmueli, D. 2006. The self-control costs of fighting the temptation to drink. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 20 (2): 154-60.

Muraven, M.; Collins, R.L. & Nienhaus, K. 2002. Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 16 (2): 113-20.

Noel, X.; Colmant, M.; Van Der Linden, M.; Bechara, A.; Bullens, Q.; Hanak, C. & Verbanck, P. 2006. Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholic patients: The role of initial orienting. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 30 (11): 1871-77.

Palfai, T.P.; Monti, PM.; Colby, S.M. & Rohsenow, D.J. 1997. Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies. Behaviour Research and Therapy 35 (1): 59-65.

Peressutti, C.; Martin-Gonzalez, J.M., Garcia-Manso, J & Mesa, D. 2009. Heart rate dynamics in different levels of Zen meditation. International Journal of Cardiology. [Epub ahead of print] Purdon, C. 1999. Thought suppression and psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (11): 1029-54.

Rawson, R. & McCann, M.J. 2006. Counselor's Treatment Manual: Matrix Intensive Outpatient Treatment for People With Stimulant Use Disorders. Rockville, MD: CSAT.

Rogers, C. 2003. Client-Centered Therapy: Its Current Practice, Implications, and Theory. New York: Constable.

Rohsenow, D.J.; Monti, P.M.; Rubonis, A.V.; Sirota, A.D.; Niaura, R.S.; Colby, S.M.; Wunschel, S.M.; Munroe, S. & Abrams, D.B. 1994. Cue reactivity as a predictor of drinking among male alcoholics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62 (3): 620-26.

Schroder, K.E. & Perrine, M.W. 2007. Covariations of emotional states and alcohol consumption: Evidence from 2 years of daily data collection. Social Science & Medicine 65 (12): 2588-2602.

Segal, Z.; Williams, J.M. & Teasdale, J.D. 2002. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression. New York: Guilford Press.

Segerstrom, S.C. & Nes, L.S. 2007. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychological Science 18 (3): 275-81.

Sinha, R. 2007. The role of stress in addiction relapse. Current Psychiatry Reports 9 (5): 388-95.

Skinner, H.A. 1982. The Drug Abuse Screening Test. Addictive Behaviors 7 (4): 363-71.

Stormark, K.M.; Field, N.P.; Hugdahl, K. & Horowitz, M. 1997. Selective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: an approach-avoidance conflict? Addictive Behaviors 22 (4): 509-19.

Tang, Y.Y.; Ma, Y.; Fan, Y.; Feng, H.; Wang, J.; Feng, S.; Lu, Q.; Hu, B.; Lin, Y.; Li, J.; Zhang, Y.; Wang, Y.; Zhou, L. & Fan, M. 2009. Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106 (22): 8865-70.

Thayer, J.F. & Lane, R.D. 2009. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2): 8188.

Thayer, J.F. & Lane, R.D. 2000. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders 61 (3): 201-16.

Tiffany, S.T. 1990. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review 97 (2): 147-68.

Tiffany, S.T.; Carter, B.L. & Singleton, E.G. 2000. Challenges in the manipulation, assessment and interpretation of craving relevant variables. Addiction 95 (Supplement 2): S177-87.

Townshend, J.M. & Duka, T. 2007. Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 31 (8): 1349-57.

Wegner, D.M. & Zanakos, S. 1994. Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality 62 (4): 616-40.

Wegner, D.M.; Schneider, D.J.; Carter, S.R., 3rd & White, T.L. 1987. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 (1): 5-13.

Wenk-Sormaz, H. 2005. Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Alternative Therapies: Health and Medicine 11 (2): 42-58.

Wenzlaff, R.M. & Wegner, D.M. 2000. Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology 51: 59-91.

Valdez, G.R. & Koob, G.F. 2004. Allostasis and dysregulation of corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y systems: Implications for the development of alcoholism. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 79 (4): 671-89.

Zgierska, A.; Rabago, D.; Zuelsdorff, M.; Coe, C.; Miller, M. & Fleming, M. 2008. Mindfulness meditation for alcohol relapse prevention: A feasibility pilot study. Journal of Addiction Medicine 2 (3): 165-73.

([dagger]) ELG was supported by Grant Number T32AT003378 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine, a Francisco J. Varela Research Grant from the Mind and Life Institute, Boulder, CO, and an Armfield-Reeves Innovation Grant from the University of North Carolina School of Social Work, Chapel Hill, NC. CAB was supported by Award Number KL2RR025746 from the National Center for Research Resources. The contents of this publication do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors would also like to acknowledge the helpful contribution of Vicki West Channon, Ph.D., and Laura Andrews, B.A. for their assistance with developing the AB dot probe task, as well as Stuart Scott, B.A. and Noah Martinson, M.S.W. for their data collection efforts. In addition, we would like to acknowledge Brant Nix, B.M.E.T. of the UNC School of Nursing Biobehavioral Laboratory for his technical assistance with psychophysiological data collection procedures. We would also like to acknowledge Benjamin Burnette for his logistical support of the project.

Eric L. Garland, Ph.D., L.C.S.W., Assistant Professor, Florida State University College of Social Work, Tallahassee, FL.

Susan A. Gaylord, Ph.D., Director, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine, Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation--Program on Integrative Medicine, Chapel Hill, NC.

Charlotte A. Boettiger, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Department of Psychology, Biomedical Research Imaging Center,& Bowles Center for Alcohol Studies, Chapel Hill, NC.

Matthew O. Howard, Ph.D., Frank Daniels Distinguished Professor, University of North Carolina--Chapel Hill School of Social Work, Chapel Hill, NC.

Please address correspondence and reprint requests to Eric L. Garland, Ph.D., L.C.S.W., College of Social Work, Florida State University, University Center, Building C, Tallahassee, FL 32306-2570. Phone: 919943-6022; email: elgarlan@gmail.com
TABLE 1
Pre-Intervention Characteristics of MORE and ASG Intervention
Groups, Total Study Participants and Program Completers

Variable                                MORE            ASG
                                      (N = 27)       (N = 26)

Length of Stay in Residential        22.4 (2.6)      22.2 (4.6)
  Program (M, SD)
Gender N (%)
  Male                               22 (81.5)       20 (76.9)
  Female                              5 (18.5)     6(23.1)
Race N (%)
  African American                   17 (62.9)       15 (57.7)
  Caucasian                           7 (25.9)       11 (42.3)
  Other                               3 (11.1)        0
Age (M, SD)                          39.9 (8.7)      40.7 (10.2)
DSM-IV Dependence Criteria (M, SD)    6.5 (0.9)       6.6 (0.9)
Income before entering TC N (%)
  <$20,000                           14 (51.9)       14 (51.9)
  $20,000-40,000                      9 (33.3)        8 (30.8)
  $41,000-60,000                      2 (7.4)         3 (11.5)
  $61,000-80,000                      1 (3.7)         0
  >$80,000                            1 (3.7)         1 (3.8)
Drinks per Day prior to              21.4 (11.9)     16.6 (9.5)
  Entering  TC (M, SD)
AUDIT (M, SD)                        32.8 (6.0)      31.8 (5.3)
DAST (M, SD)                         20.7 (5.1)      21.2 (5.8)
Psychiatric Symptoms:
  Depression                          4.9 (4.6)       5.0 (4.4)
  Anxiety                             5.8 (3.8)       5.0 (4.3)
  Hostility                           4.0 (3.6)       4.3 (3.8)
  Interpersonal Sensitivity           4.4 (3.8)       3.9 (3.4)
Trauma:
  # of Times Seen Someone             3.0 (3.2)       2.7 (2.7)
    Killed in Person
  # of Times Hurt or in Danger        6.0 (11.4)      6.7 (12.3)
    of Being Killed
  # of Times Hit by Someone           7.3 (6.9)       9.1 (11.4)
    with Intent to Hurt
  # of Times Mugged                   2.4 (2.9)       1.5 (1.5)
  # of Times Attacked by a
    Weapon or by Someone
    with Intent to Kill               2.5 (2.2)       2.3 (1.7)

Variable                                MORE          Total
                                      (N = 27)      (N = 53)

Length of Stay in Residential        22.4 (2.6)    22.3 (3.7)
  Program (M, SD)
Gender N (%)
  Male                               22 (81.5)     42 (79.2)
  Female                              5 (18.5)     11 (20.8)
Race N (%)
  African American                   17 (62.9)     32 (60.4)
  Caucasian                           7 (25.9)     18 (34.0)
  Other                               3 (11.1)      3 (5.6)
Age (M, SD)                          39.9 (8.7)    40.3 (9.4)
DSM-IV Dependence Criteria (M, SD)    6.5 (0.9)     6.5 (1.0)
Income before entering TC N (%)
  <$20,000                           14 (51.9)     28 (52.8)
  $20,000-40,000                      9 (33.3)     17 (32.1)
  $41,000-60,000                      2 (7.4)       5 (9.4)
  $61,000-80,000                      1 (3.7)       1 (1.9)
  >$80,000                            1 (3.7)       2 (3.8)
Drinks per Day prior to              21.4 (11.9)   19.0 (10.9)
  Entering  TC (M, SD)
AUDIT (M, SD)                        32.8 (6.0)    32.3 (5.6)
DAST (M, SD)                         20.7 (5.1)    21.0 (5.4)
Psychiatric Symptoms:
  Depression                          4.9 (4.6)     4.9 (4.4)
  Anxiety                             5.8 (3.8)     5.4 (4.1)
  Hostility                           4.0 (3.6)     4.2 (3.6)
  Interpersonal Sensitivity           4.4 (3.8)     4.2 (3.6)
Trauma:
  # of Times Seen Someone             3.0 (3.2)     2.9 (2.9)
    Killed in Person
  # of Times Hurt or in Danger        6.0 (11.4)    6.4 (11.7)
    of Being Killed
  # of Times Hit by Someone           7.3 (6.9)     8.1 (9.3)
    with Intent to Hurt
  # of Times Mugged                   2.4 (2.9)     2.0 (2.3)
  # of Times Attacked by a
    Weapon or by Someone
    with Intent to Kill               2.5 (2.2)     2.4 (2.0)

Variable                                MORE       Completers
                                      (N = 27)      (N = 37)

Length of Stay in Residential        22.4 (2.6)    22.05 (2.4)
  Program (M, SD)
Gender N (%)
  Male                               22 (81.5)     28 (75.7)
  Female                              5 (18.5)      9 (24.3)
Race N (%)
  African American                   17 (62.9)     24 (64.9)
  Caucasian                           7 (25.9)     12 (32.4)
  Other                               3 (11.1)      1 (2.7)
Age (M, SD)                          39.9 (8.7)    40.9 (9.5)
DSM-IV Dependence Criteria (M, SD)    6.5 (0.9)     6.4 (1.0)
Income before entering TC N (%)
  <$20,000                           14 (51.9)     21 (56.8)
  $20,000-40,000                      9 (33.3)     11 (29.7)
  $41,000-60,000                      2 (7.4)       3 (8.1)
  $61,000-80,000                      1 (3.7)       1 (2.7)
  >$80,000                            1 (3.7)       1 (2.7)
Drinks per Day prior to              21.4 (11.9)   18.7 (11.2)
  Entering  TC (M, SD)
AUDIT (M, SD)                        32.8 (6.0)    32.08 (5.4)
DAST (M, SD)                         20.7 (5.1)    20.4 (4.7)
Psychiatric Symptoms:
  Depression                          4.9 (4.6)     4.9 (4.9)
  Anxiety                             5.8 (3.8)     4.9 (4.2)
  Hostility                           4.0 (3.6)     3.9 (3.8)
  Interpersonal Sensitivity           4.4 (3.8)     4.2 (3.9)
Trauma:
  # of Times Seen Someone             3.0 (3.2)     2.9 (2.9)
    Killed in Person
  # of Times Hurt or in Danger        6.0 (11.4)    6.4 (11.7)
    of Being Killed
  # of Times Hit by Someone           7.3 (6.9)     8.1 (9.3)
    with Intent to Hurt
  # of Times Mugged                   2.4 (2.9)     2.0 (2.3)
  # of Times Attacked by a
    Weapon or by Someone
    with Intent to Kill               2.5 (2.2)     2.4 (2.0)

Note: All of the variables listed above were analyzed with
one-way ANOVAs and chi-square tests. No significant baseline
differences between treatment groups or completers and
noncompleters were found.

TABLE 2
Session-Specific Description of MORE * and ASG Interventions

Session   MORE                            ASG

1         Discussion of mindfulness and   Discussion of triggers for
          automatic drinking;             alcohol dependence relapse
          mindfulness of craving;
          mindful breathing and body
          scan

2         Discussion of cognitive         Discussion of justifications
          reappraisal; practice of        for relapse and using alcohol
          mindful decentering and         to mask one's emotions
          mindful breathing

3         Discussion of attentional       Discussion of how
          re-orienting as means of        participants cope with
          coping with negative emotions   emotions of anger and sadness
          and craving; mindful
          breathing; mindfulness of
          perceptions & sensations

4         Discussion of craving;          Discussion of how
          practice of "urge surfing,"     participants cope with
          mindfulness of craving, and     craving and managing
          contemplation of negative       compulsive behavior
          consequences of drinking;
          imaginal alcohol cue
          exposure; mindful breathing
          practice

5         Discussion of the               Discussion of stress and
          relationship of the stress      methods participants use to
          response to craving; imaginal   relax without alcohol
          stress exposure; mindful
          breathing

6         Discussion of thought           Discussion of the attempt to
          suppression, aversion, and      maintain control and loss of
          attachment; exercise in the     control
          futility of thought
          suppression; mindful
          breathing and acceptance

7         Discussion of the deleterious   Discussion of physical health
          effects of alcohol on the       promotion and ways to
          body; mindful interoceptive     recreate without alcohol
          awareness; mindful walking

8         Discussion of relational        Discussion of codependence
          triggers for relapse; mindful   relationships and the
          communication; compassion       challenge of forming
          meditation                      friendships without alcohol

9         Discussion of                   Discussion of spirituality
          interdependence, meaning, and
          spirituality; meditation on
          interdependence; mindful
          breathing

10        Review; discussion of how to    Reflection on the support
          maintain mindfulness            group experience; discussion
          practice; development of        of the recovery process
          mindful relapse prevention
          plan; imaginal rehearsal of
          mindful relapse prevention;
          mindful breathing

* MORE is adapted from Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for
Depression (Segal et al. 2002) and tailored to apply mindfulness
principles to addiction-related topics.

TABLE 3
Intervention Effects on Clinical and Attentional Variables

                                             Means

Variables                              Pre            Post

Perceived Stress
  MORE (n=18)                      15.6 (4.7)     10.8 (5.3)
  ASG (n=19)                       16.0 (7.6)     14.5 (5.8)
Thought Suppression
  MORE (n=18)                      53.6 (8.7)     50.1 (7.9)
  ASG (n=19)                       50.9 (11.2)    53.5 (9.4)
Mindfulness
  MORE (n=17)                     108.3 (12.3)   109.8 (14.6)
  ASG (n=19)                      115.9 (15.1)   118.3 (17.9)
Craving
  MORE (n=18)                       4.7 (5.5)      4.6 (5.3)
  ASG (n=19)                        4.9 (4.4)      3.2 (3.6)
Impaired Alcohol
Response Inhibition
  MORE (n=18)                       7.8 (5.5)      4.9 (4.9)
  ASG (n=19)                        6.2 (4.9)      5.3 (4.2)
Global Psychiatric Symptoms
  MORE (n=18)                      42.7 (36.4)    19.6 (12.5)
  ASG (n=19)                       46.7 (33.0)    31.8 (21.4)

                                  Pre           Mid           Post

200ms Approach AB
  MORE (n=9)                   7.2 (6.9)     10.7 (13.9)    2.9 (21.9)
  ASG (n=9)                   13.5 (8.2)     -9.0 (19.9)   17.6 (17.2)
2000ms Approach AB
  MORE (n=7)                  12.6 (12.1)     3.6 (23.7)    2.3 (21.9)
  ASG (n=7)                   12.9 (9.1)    -12.8 (10.9)    1.7 (10.4)

                                                  F-test

                                                           [eta]
Variables                                    df     F    [o.sup.2]

Perceived Stress              Time           1    18.11     .34
  MORE (n=18)                 Group x Time   1     5.06     .13
  ASG (n=19)
Thought Suppression           Time           1      .11     .00
  MORE (n=18)                 Group x Time   1     4.26     .11
  ASG (n=19)
Mindfulness                   Time           1      .98     .02
  MORE (n=17)                 Group x Time   1      .05     .002
  ASG (n=19)
Craving                       Time           1     1.36     .04
  MORE (n=18)                 Group x Time   1     1.05     .03
  ASG (n=19)
Impaired Alcohol              Time           1     3.43     .07
Response Inhibition
  MORE (n=18)                 Group x Time   1      .88     .03
  ASG (n=19)
Global Psychiatric            Time           1    10.83     .24
Symptoms
  MORE (n=18)                 Group x Time   1      .50     .01
  ASG (n=19)

                                             df    F       [eta]

200ms Approach AB             Time           1     1.76     .10
  MORE (n=9)                  Group x Time   2     4.76     .23
  ASG (n=9)
2000ms Approach AB            Time           1     4.54     .27
  MORE (n=7)                  Group x Time   1     1.07     .08
  ASG (n=7)

                              F-test

Variables                       p

Perceived Stress              <.00
  MORE (n=18)                  .03
  ASG (n=19)
Thought Suppression            .74
  MORE (n=18)                  .04
  ASG (n=19)
Mindfulness                    .33
  MORE (n=17)                  .81
  ASG (n=19)
Craving                        .25
  MORE (n=18)                  .31
  ASG (n=19)
Impaired Alcohol               .09
Response Inhibition
  MORE (n=18)                  .35
  ASG (n=19)
Global Psychiatric Symptoms    .002
  MORE (n=18)                  .48
  ASG (n=19)

                                p

200ms Approach AB              .20
  MORE (n=9)                   .03
  ASG (n=9)
2000ms Approach AB             .02
  MORE (n=7)                   .36
  ASG (n=7)
COPYRIGHT 2010 Haight-Ashbury Publications
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2010 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Garland, Eric L.; Gaylord, Susan A.; Boettiger, Charlotte A.; Howard, Matthew O.
Publication:Journal of Psychoactive Drugs
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:1USA
Date:Jun 1, 2010
Words:12355
Previous Article:Sweet preference, sugar addiction and the familial history of alcohol dependence: shared neural pathways and genes.
Next Article:Addiction and related disorders.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2021 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters |