Printer Friendly

Fear of losing makes people choke when stakes are high.

Washington, May 10 ( ANI ): Researchers including one of an Indian origin have suggested that when there are high financial incentives to succeed, people can become so afraid of losing their potentially lucrative reward that their performance suffers.

It is a somewhat unexpected conclusion. After all, you would think that the more people are paid, the harder they will work, and the better they will do their jobs-until they reach the limits of their skills.

That notion tends to hold true when the stakes are low, said Vikram Chib, a postdoctoral scholar at the California Institute of Technology (Caltech).

Previous research, however, has shown that if you pay people too much, their performance actually declines.

Some experts have attributed this decline to too much motivation: they think that, faced with the prospect of earning an extra chunk of cash, you might get so excited that you will fail to do the task properly.

But now, after looking at brain-scan data of volunteers performing a specific motor task, the Caltech team said that what actually happens is that you become worried about losing your potential prize. The researchers also found that the more someone is afraid of loss, the worse they perform.

In the study, each participant was asked to control a virtual object on a screen by moving an index finger that had a tracking device attached to it. The virtual object consisted of two weighted balls connected by a spring. The task was to place the object, which stretched and contracted as a weighted spring would in real life, into a square target within two seconds.

The researchers controlled for individual skill levels by customizing the size of the target so that everyone would have the same success rate. That way, people who happened to be really good or bad at this task would not skew the data.

After a training period, the subjects were asked to perform the task while inside an fMRI machine, which measures blood flow in the brain-a proxy for brain activity, since wherever a brain is active, it needs extra oxygen, and thus a larger volume of blood. By monitoring blood flow, the researchers can pinpoint areas of the brain that turn on when a particular task is performed.

The task began with the researchers offering the participants a randomized range of rewards-from 0 dollars to 100 dollars -if they could successfully place the object into the square within the time limit. At the end of hundreds of trials-each with varying reward amounts-the participant was given their reward, based on the result of just one of the trials, picked at random.

As expected, the team found that performance improved as the incentives increased-but only when the cash reward amounts were at the low end of the spectrum. Once the rewards passed a certain threshold, which depended on the individual, performance began to fall off.

Incentives are known to activate a part of your brain called the ventral striatum, Chib stated; the researchers thus expected to see the ventral striatum become increasingly active as they bumped up the prizes. And if the conventional thought were correct-that the reason for the observed performance decline was over-motivation-they would expect the striatum to continue showing a lot of activation when the incentives became high enough for performance to suffer.

What they found, instead, was that when the participants were shown their potential rewards, activity in the striatum did indeed increase with rising incentives. But once the volunteers started doing the task, striatal activity decreased with rising incentives. They also noticed that the less activity they saw in a participant's striatum, the worse that person performed on the task.

Other studies have shown that decreasing striatal activity is related to fear or aversion to loss, Chib noted.

"When people see the incentive that they're being offered, they initially encode it as a gain. But when they're actually doing the task, the thing that causes them to perform poorly is that they worry about losing a potential incentive they haven't even received yet," he explained.

"We're showing loss aversion even though there are no explicit losses anywhere in the task-that's very strange and something you really wouldn't expect," he added.

To further test their hypothesis, Chib and his colleagues decided to measure how loss-averse each participant was. They had the participants play a coin-flip game in which there was an equal chance they could win or lose varying amounts of money.

Each participant was offered varying potential win-loss amounts, and then given the opportunity to either accept each possible gamble or decline it. The win-loss ratio at which the subjects chose to take the gamble provided a measure of how loss-averse each person was; someone willing to gamble even when they might win or lose 20 dollars is less loss-averse than someone who is only willing to gamble if they can win 20 dollars but only lose 5 dollars.

Once the numbers had been crunched and compared to the original experiment, it turned out that the more averse a participant was, the worse they did on the task when the stakes were high. And for a particularly loss-aversive person, the threshold at which their performance started to decline did not have to be very high.

"If you're more loss-averse, it really hurts you. You're going to reach peak performance at a lower incentive level, and your performance is also going to be worse for higher incentives," Chib, lead author of the study, said.

John O'Doherty, professor of psychology and coauthor of the paper added, "Previously, it's been shown that the ventral striatum is involved in mediating performance increases in response to rising incentives."

"But our study shows that changes in activity in this same region can, under certain situations, also lead to worsening performance," he stated.

These findings, the researchers say, might be used to develop new ways to motivate people to perform better or to train them to be less loss-averse.

The study results were published in the May 10 issue of the journal Neuron. ( ANI )

]]>

Copyright 2012 aninews.in All rights reserved.

Provided by Syndigate.info an Albawaba.com company
COPYRIGHT 2012 Al Bawaba (Middle East) Ltd.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2012 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Publication:Asian News International
Date:May 10, 2012
Words:1021
Previous Article:Massive black holes shut down star formation in distant galaxies.
Next Article:Early music lessons may boost babies' brains.

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2021 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters