Printer Friendly

Dream recall frequencies and dream content in Wilson's disease with and without REM sleep behaviour disorder: a neurooneirologic study.

1. Introduction

Dream content reflects waking-life experiences, current concerns, and waking-life symptoms [1]. Since the times of the healing temples of the god Asclepius in ancient Greece, dream reports were used in medical diagnostics [2]. In modern medicine only the diagnosis of the REM sleep behaviour disorder (RBD) is based on the clinical history of dream enactment behaviour, presumed to occur during REM sleep, in addition to the polysomnographic documentation of REM sleep without atonia (RWA) [3]. As a critical feature of dreaming in RBD, in both its idiopathic and synucleinopathy-associated forms, higher amounts of aggressive dream contents have been reported [4-6]. However, it is not clear whether dreams of RBD patients contain in general more movement or aggression or only those dreams, which are acted out [7-9].

The main manifestations of Wilson's disease (WD) are hepatic dysfunction and a broad spectrum of movement disorders with parkinsonian, dystonic, ataxic, and choreatic characteristics. Cognitive limitations, especially in the executive domain [10], as well as depression are frequent in WD [11] and might contribute to altered dreaming. On the other hand, in some movement disorder conditions, physical motor disability has been shown to affect dream content very little [12,13], as these patients generally dream of themselves without limitations of their movement abilities. In addition, RBD has been recently described also in WD [14]. Polysomnography parameters and sleep quality have been shown to be significantly worse in WD, both with and without RBD, as compared to healthy controls [15]. Diseases, which structurally affect the brain like stroke, traumatic brain injury, neoplasm, Parkinson's disease, Alzheimer's disease, fatal familial insomnia, and sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, have been shown to have impact on dreaming [16,17]. In a study in WD, a single question on "presence of vivid dreams" was more often answered positively in patients than in controls, but the difference was not statistically significant [18]. A questionnaire study on sleep in WD found that nightmares were reported less often by WD patients than by randomly selected controls and less often by male than by female WD patients [19].

As no data on dream frequencies and content in WD have been published, this study was performed in a prospective design to characterize dreaming in WD and in RBD in WD. Therefore, we investigated dream and nightmare recall frequencies and dream content in WD patients versus healthy, age- and sex-matched controls and in WD patients with RBD (WD + RBD) versus WD patients without RbD (WD - RBD).

We hypothesized the following. (1) In WD, dream recall frequency (DRF) and dream word count are reduced and dream content is more negative as compared to healthy controls due to the cognitive impairment in patients with WD. (2) Dreams of patients with WD show increased concerns about motor disability as compared to healthy controls as this is a major impairment in WD. (3) Patients with WD + RBD have more negative and aggressive dream content than patients with WD - RBD.

2. Methods

2.1. Patients and Healthy Controls. Patients were recruited from the movement disorders outpatient clinic at the Sao Paulo University Hospital, Sao Paulo, Brazil. Age- and sex-matched controls were recruited from medical students and administrative hospital staff. The local ethics committee of the University of Sao Paulo approved the study; written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants. All participants underwent extended and structured face-to-face interviews and clinical examinations by two board certified neurologists.

In a prior publication, we have reported on sleep characteristics of a cohort of 41 patients with WD, including five patients with WD and RBD [15]. Diagnostic procedures and the video-polysomnography documented sleep disorder have been published before [15]. The present study reports on the dream characteristics of this cohort. Due to their WD, three patients, one of them a male patient with RBD, were incapable of communicating on their dream characteristics and had to be excluded from the present study. Thirty-eight WD patients of all severity of illness levels but with preserved capacity to communicate were included; Unified Wilson's Disease Rating Scale (UWDRS) was 68.82 [+ or -] 41.12 (range 18-186). Present WD manifestation was clearly predominantly affecting the brain (UWDRS neurological subscore 50.76 [+ or -] 34.22) with almost no hepatic symptoms (UWDRS 5.87 [+ or -] 3.02). WD was diagnosed biochemically and/or genetically, according to established guidelines [20]. RBD was diagnosed according to ICSD-3 criteria (International classification of sleep disorders, 3rd ed.) [3]. RWA was determined in 32 WD patients by quantification of submental and bilateral flexor digitorum superficialis electromyogram in a whole night video-polysomnography [21]; 4 out of 38 (10%) WD patients fulfilled criteria for RBD. At examination, 12 WD patients suffered symptoms of psychiatric diseases: depression (n = 11), personality disorder (n = 3), anxiety disorder (n = 2), and attention deficit disorder (n = 1). 11 WD patients took a psychotropic medication: antidepressants (n = 9), benzodiazepines (n = 5; i.e., clonazepam n = 3 [range 1-8mg/d], lorazepam n = 1, and clobazam n = 1), and neuroleptics (n = 2). Apart from one patient on amitriptyline 75 mg/d due to depression and another one on clonazepam 0.5 mg/d, WD + RBD patients did not suffer from psychiatric diseases and were free of a psychotropic medication.

41 age- and sex-matched, healthy controls were free of all of the following: neurological and hepatic diseases, treatment requiring sleep disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, abuse of alcohol, and illegal drugs for the lifetime; any other psychiatric disorders and intake of antidepressant, benzodiazepine, and neuroleptic medication during the last year. 18 controls underwent the same video-polysomnography protocol as the patients.

2.2. Cognitive, Affective, and Sleep Questionnaires. Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised [22] is a brief test sensitive to the early stages of dementia, such as mild cognitive impairment. It examines five cognitive domains: attention/orientation (18 points), memory (26), verbal fluency (14), language (26), and visuospatial abilities (16), with a maximum total score of 100. Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised has a very good reliability with an alpha coefficient = 0.8. Cut-offs were defined at 88 (sensitivity = 0.94, specificity = 0.89) and at 82 (sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 1.0). The likelihood of dementia was 100 : 1 at a cut-off of 82. To control for symptoms of depression and for sleep quality, the Beck Depression Inventory [23] and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index [24] were implemented.

2.3. Dream Questionnaires

2.3.1. Dream and Nightmare Recall Frequencies. Overall DRF during the last months was examined by a seven-point rating scale (0 = never, 1 = less than once a month, 2 = about once a month, 3 = twice or three times a month, 4 = about once a week, 5 = several times a week, and 6 = almost every morning) [25]. This scale showed a retest reliability for an average interval of 55 days of r = 0.85 (n = 198; [26]). Using the class means, the scale was recoded to obtain units of mornings with dream recall per week (0 [right arrow] 0, 1 [right arrow] 0.125, 2 [right arrow] 0.25, 3 [right arrow] 0.625, 4 [right arrow] 1.0, 5 [right arrow] 3.5, and 6 [right arrow] 6.5). To measure the nightmare frequency, an eight-point rating scale [27] was presented ("how often did you experience nightmares during the last months?" 0 = never, 1 = less than once a year, 2 = about once a year, 3 = about 2 to 4 times a year, 4 = about once a month, 5 = about 2 to 3 times a month, 6 = about once a week, and 7 = several times a week). The term "nightmares" was not further defined in the instructions. To obtain units in frequency per month, class means were used to recode the scales (0 [right arrow] 0, 1 [right arrow] 0.042, 2 [right arrow] 0.083, 3 [right arrow] 0.25, 4 [right arrow] 1.0, 5 [right arrow] 2.5, 6 [right arrow] 4.0, and 7 [right arrow] 12.0). The class means were deduced from the categories, not by mathematical formula.

2.3.2. Relationship between WD and Dreaming. Patients were asked to answer the following question in written form: "did you ever observe any relationship between your Wilson's disease and your dreams?" We strictly avoided inducing any possible answer beyond the free response of the participant.

2.3.3. RBD Questionnaire-Hong Kong (RBDQ-HK). The 13item self-reported RBDQ-HK has been developed to diagnose and to monitor RBD [28]. The first five items ask dream and nightmare features. The lifetime items can be answered in three categories: "do not know," "no," or "yes", coded as 0, 0, or 1. Recent 1-year frequency items are rated on a five-point scale (0 = none, 1 = yes/once or few times per year, 2 = once or few times per month, 3 = 1-2 times per week, and 4 = 3 times or above per week). The psychometric properties of the RBDQ-HK were sensitivity 82.2%, specificity 86.9%, positive predictive value 86.3%, negative predictive value 83.0%, and high internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha coefficients 0.90 for the overall scale and 0.86 for the dreams-related items) and test-retest reliability (test-retest coefficients 0.89 for lifetime and 0.80 for recent 1-year frequency scales).

2.4. Last Remembered Dream. The participants were asked to write down the last dream they could remember with as many details and as completely as possible [29]. They were to describe the situation in the dream, whether known or unknown, the persons, their sex, age, and relationship, and if there were animals. They were also asked to describe their feelings during the dream and if they were positive or negative.

2.5. Dream Diary. Subjects were required to keep a dream diary during a two-week period [30]. They were asked to write down, in the morning, immediately upon awakening and as completely as possible, the dreams they had experienced during the past night. Participants were also asked to mark if they did not remember any dream or if they remembered to have dreamt but had forgotten the content. After having written the dream, the participants were asked to estimate the intensity of positive and negative emotions on two four-point scales: 0 = none, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, and 3 = strong. The participants were asked to write down dream reports on up to five mornings. At the remaining mornings until the fourteenth morning, participants had to mark only if they remembered the content of a dream from the past night or not.

2.6. Dream Content Analysis. Dream content was analyzed using scales from Schredl et al. [31] and Schredl and Engelhardt [30]: realism/bizarreness (0 = realistic to 3 = two or more bizarre elements within the dream), positive and negative emotions (two different four-point scales: 0 = none, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, and 3 = strong), and the number of dream characters. The occurrences of verbal and physical interaction, verbal and physical aggression, characteristics of aggressors, health-related topics, professional environment, leisure, laboratory references, problems, depression, and death related topics were coded binary (1 = present or 0 = not present). Five additional binary scales were constructed for the purpose of this study: reference to and movements of legs and arms, physical activity, and restriction of movements.

The interrater reliability of these scales is satisfactory [32]: realism/bizarreness r = 0.765, positive emotions r = 0.642, negative emotions r = 0.825 (all Spearman rank correlations), for occurrence of verbal interaction judges reached 87% exact agreement.

2.7. Procedure. The questionnaires were filled in during the interview and subsequently discussed and corrected if necessary. The RBDQ-HK was implemented at a second interview; the interval was 4.3 [+ or -] 1.7 months. The handwritten dream reports were transcribed. Word count was used to determine dream length. The dream reports were rearranged randomly and were rated along the scales described in the dream content analysis section by a, for the diagnoses blind, native Portuguese-speaking judge (J.P.).

2.8. Statistical Analysis. SPSS 20.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL) and SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) were used for statistical analyses. To adjust for multiple dream reports from one participant, mixed models were used according to the variables' measurement levels. We used general linear mixed models for Gaussian data and generalized linear mixed models for ordinal data (ordinal logistic regression with mixed effects). The marginal approach of generalized estimating equations (GEE) was used for analyzing correlated binary data. To compare WD + RBD versus WD - RBD groups, means of emotional scales of each patient and numbers of dreams with aggressive content of each patient were compared in a Mann-Whitney U test. Levels of significance were set to alpha 0.05. Being a study of mainly exploratory character, Bonferroni correction was not applied.

3. Results

3.1. Wilson's Disease Patients Compared to Healthy Controls. Compared to controls, cognitive performance of WD patients measured by the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised was significantly lower, especially in the domains of verbal fluency, visuospatial abilities, and memory (Table 1). Also the emotional state in the Beck Depression Inventory and the sleep quality in the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index were significantly worse.

Patients with WD showed a significantly reduced dream word count compared to controls. In almost all of the other dream dimensions examined, WD patients showed similar dream characteristics as controls (Tables 2 and 3). DRF, both retrospectively asked and prospectively determined in a two-week diary, and nightmare frequencies were not different between WD patients and controls. In the dream content dimension, only retrospective lifetime questions on violent or aggressive dream content and on frightening and horrifying content (RBDQ-HK4 and 5) were significantly different; they were less often answered positively by WD patients than by controls.

26 of 38 WD patients (68.42%) and 35 of 41 controls (85.37%, n.s.) wrote at least one dream report (Table 3). The total number of dream reports from patients was 112 (last dreams n = 21, diary dreams n = 91) and from controls 146 (32 and 114, resp.). Dream report contents of WD patients and controls did not differ significantly throughout all the subjectively and judge-rated categories. This also included no difference in frequencies of motor activity themes in dreams of WD patients.

Interviewed before writing the dream reports on whether they already had observed any relationship between WD and dreaming, 15 patients answered affirmatively. Of these 15 patients, only four patients reported WD itself as an explicit theme of a dream. One patient had cried in a dream because of being newly diagnosed with the disease, one had had repeated dreams of losing daily life capacities while starting WD treatment, one had dreamt of never reaching an improvement of the disease, and one had repeatedly dreamt of being able to eat chocolate (which patients have to abstain from because of its copper contents) and of having healthy children without WD. Six patients had had dreams that they could move the body without any disabling condition (being entirely like before the disease; dancing; travelling; speaking and walking normally; speaking and making love as before; going out with friends at night), while two patients had had dreams on motor disability (one dreamt of losing his legs in an accident; another, a teacher, had repeated dreams in the beginning of the disease of having lost her ability to speak and saw herself standing in front of her class having lost her voice).

3.2. WD Patients with RBD versus WD Patients without RBD. The implemented questionnaires showed no significant difference between WD patients with and without RBD in cognitive performance and emotional state; sleep quality was considerably worse in WD + RBD, though not reaching significance, probably due to the low number of patients (Table 1).

Compared to WD - RBD, patients with WD + RBD rated consistently a significantly higher nightmare frequency for both the last months and the last year, determined with two different scales at different points in time (Table 2). WD patients with RBD also reported to have significantly more dreams with violent or aggressive content and showed a tendency towards more dreams with frightening and horrifying content during the past year.

The total number of dream reports from four patients with WD + RBD was 18 (last dreams n = 3, diary dreams n = 15); from 22 patients with WD - RBD it was 94 (18 and 76, resp.; Table 4). Further 12 patients with WD RBD did not remember dream content in the prospective part of the study. Dream contents of WD patients with RBD showed significantly more self-rated negative emotions, as a tendency also more negative emotions in the judge-rating, and significantly more aggression, in particular more verbal aggression towards the dream ego. The number of reports per patient and the word count per dream report were not different.

3.3. Dream Examples from the Diaries. Female patient with WD and RBD, age 20, wrote:
   I was in a school. There were many classrooms.
   The walls were painted in a certain green, which
   reminded me both of a hospital, when I was a
   child, in Monte Verde, and of my elementary
   school. The school desks were made of wood and
   I was sitting at a table, looking there at two
   empty pictures in the background, one black and
   one white. The bell rings, sounds of rushing, but
   instead of children, as I expected, appeared my
   mother, my friend, and many male nurses. They
   grabbed me and threw me on the floor (my mother
   cried at the door). I screamed that I wasn't crazy.
   They injected something into my neck and into
   the base of my spine. My friend tried to calm me
   down, but I went on screaming and flailing. They
   threw me in a car that then right away turned into
   a room. I cried and didn't see anybody, only myself
   (from above). Note: I woke up with scratches on
   my arm.


Female patient with WD without RBD, age 34, wrote:
   I dreamt that I was at Grandma's house. It was
   daytime and the weather was very cold. The house
   was under renovation and I helped painting a
   wall. It became each time colder and it seemed that
   the wall was doubling in size. It was as if I had not
   yet gotten the disease, as if all was still as before. I
   remember that I felt very good about that.


Male patient with WD without RBD, age 31, wrote:
   I dreamt that I traveled with my girlfriend to
   Bahia and we went to the house of my grandmother
   who lives there. My girlfriend and me
   were going to the parties there, because we had
   gone there during the time of Christmas holidays
   and we danced normally, as if I had no physical
   problem at all.


4. Discussion

We report first quantitative data on dreaming in WD and in WD with RBD. WD patients showed a significant lower dream word count as compared to age- and sex-matched, healthy controls, but no differences in dream recall. There were very few significant differences between WD patients and healthy controls regarding dream quality and dream content. Interestingly, the only significant group differences in dream characteristics were lifetime occurrences of violent/aggressive and frightening/horrifying dream contents, which WD patients reported significantly less frequently. The subgroup of WD patients with RBD reported significantly more aggressive dream contents, both retrospectively in the questionnaires and prospectively in the diary, and a significantly higher nightmare frequency than WD patients without RBD.

Unexpectedly, we did not find significant changes in DRF in the presence of clear neurological deficits and low ratings in visuospatial abilities, verbal fluency, memory, mood, and sleep quality. Most of our patients were under stable clinical and treatment conditions for already years and most of them had reached an improvement of the initial WD deficits. Considering the existing literature on dreaming under chronic cerebral lesions showing decreases in DRF initially after brain lesions [33], but also that DRF can recover to prelesion levels in the course of time [34], it might be supposed that dreaming had adapted to chronic deficits of the disease. In order to test this hypothesis, it would be necessary to study dream recall during the initial phase of WD and during the following course of successful treatment.

The reduced dream word count in this WD group can be attributed to cognitive deficits in verbal fluency and memory as previous research [35] showed that diary dream length is strongly related to verbal memory. That is, the cognitive deficits in WD affect the process of recalling the dream in full detail. We cannot exclude the possibility that these cognitive deficits might have caused also other influences on dream characteristics of the patients. The data on dream emotions did not confirm our hypothesis as we expected more negatively toned dreams in WD patients due to the burden of their disease, indicated by the higher Beck Depression Inventory scores.

The WD patients reported less lifetime occurrences of violent/aggressive and frightening/horrifying dream contents than controls, a finding that is in line with a previous study [19] showing reduced nightmare frequency in WD patients compared to controls. As these characteristics did not emerge in the prospectively collected diary dreams and the nightmare frequency scale, this finding might reflect a development of dream emotions over the course of the illness.

In our WD sample, movement ratings as well as the other content dimensions investigated were normal. The physical handicap was almost not represented in the dreams. As research indicate that congenital paraplegic patients [12], amputees [36], patients with ataxic disorders and Parkinson's disease [13], and patients with lesional hemiplegia, cortical blindness, and aphasia [16] have dreams with an intact body image, one might speculate that dreams include an innate body image that is not related to waking-life experience. A recent study in amputees [37] indicates that also waking-life experiences like phantom pain can affect the body image in dreams. It would be interesting to study the subjectively perceived impairment in patients with WD and to relate this variable to the percentage of dreams with a physical handicap. The subjective reports of the patients regarding their perceived effect of WD on dreaming indicate such a relation between waking-life and dreaming.

The significant differences in regard to nightmare frequency and aggressive dream contents between the subgroup of WD patients with and without RBD are in accordance with most dream studies in RBD [4-6, 38, 39], even though one study [9] in idiopathic RBD found no heightened aggressiveness in diary dreams. One has to keep in mind, however, that the higher nightmare frequency and more aggressive content might be an artifact, because RBD patients are awaken from these dreams due to their body movements and might recall these dreams more easily; controlled studies with lab awakenings do not show these strong differences [8]. In order to follow up these preliminary findings, it would be interesting to carry out sleep laboratory studies with REM awakenings.

Atypically, all our RBD patients were women. Even though this may be explained by the higher prevalence of women among young RBD patients [40], it is a possible confounder for dream word count and content analysis. In our sample, however, number of dreams and word count did not differ between RBD and non-RBD patients. As women usually have less aggressive dreams than men [6, 41] the finding of aggressive dream content in our female RBD patients is even more meaningful. It is possible that the true frequency of RBD in WD might even be higher than in our cohort, as patients with cognitive deficits may not be aware of RBD symptoms. This possibility gains support by the finding of nine WD patients (4 males, 5 females) in our cohort, who did not fulfill clinical criteria of RBD, but showed increased RWA (two SD above the mean of healthy controls) [15]. To what extent dreaming, on the background of increased RWA, is necessary to trigger off motor behaviour during REM sleep is an open question in the pathophysiology of RBD. In the present ICSD-3 criteria for RBD [3], dream enactment behaviour is not a necessary condition for the diagnosis of RBD. Excessive RWA with repeated, PSG documented motor behaviour during REM sleep fulfill this definition of RBD. Whether such events of RBD are really free of dreaming or if these patients have only no awareness of their dream mentation, for example, due to dementia or other cognitive limits, is difficult to differentiate. On the other hand, RWA does not necessarily produce REM sleep motor behaviour. To better elucidate these connections, it would be of high interest to have solid data on DRF and dream content in various collectives of patients with only RWA, with REM sleep motor behaviour, and with fully developed RBD. This might shed more light on the significance of dreaming in the development of RBD. Another study on WD patients, implementing an RBD sleep questionnaire, found "clinical symptoms suggesting the possibility of RBD" in even 47.3% and "vivid dreams" in 23.6% of these patients [18]. However, the low number of WD patients with RBD and the low number of reported dreams available, as well as possible medication influences, are clear limits of the present study, so that the findings should be cautiously interpreted.

To summarize, the first study systematically investigating dreaming in WD showed that RBD in WD had a clear influence on dreaming and that the influence of WD on dreaming was minor. Nevertheless, the finding of reduced dream length indicated that cognitive deficits in WD have an effect on dream recall. Further studies, especially longitudinal observations from early diagnosis to successful treatment, should be carried out. In order to shed more light on the effect of waking-life symptoms on dream content, it would be advisable to elicit the subjectively experienced burden of the disease in these patients by using questionnaires or diaries and to relate this to dream content. The interesting findings in our small group of WD patients with RBD should be complemented with larger samples. Overall, the findings indicate that dreaming is affected by a severe neurological disease like Wilson's disease and that it is important to follow these patients in longitudinal designs.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/2983205

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Acknowledgment

This work is supported by the Sao Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP, Fundacao de Amparo a Pesquisa do Estado de Sao Paulo) Grant 2012/05403-3.

References

[1] M. Schredl, "Continuity in studying the continuity hypothesis of dreaming is needed," International Journal of Dream Research, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 1-8, 2012.

[2] R. Rousselle, "Healing cults in antiquity: the dream cures of Asclepius of Epidaurus," Journal of Psychohistory, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 339-352,1985.

[3] American Academy of Sleep Medicine, International Classification of Sleep Disorders, American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Darien, 1ll, USA, 3rd edition, 2014.

[4] C. H. Schenck, T. D. Hurwitz, and M. W. Mahowald, "REM sleep behaviour disorder: an update on a series of 96 patients and a review of the world literature," Journal of Sleep Research, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 224-231, 1993.

[5] M. L. Fantini, A. Corona, S. Clerici, and L. Ferini-Strambi, "Aggressive dream content without daytime aggressiveness in REM sleep behavior disorder," Neurology, vol. 65, no. 7, pp. 1010-1015, 2005.

[6] L. L. Borek, R. Kohn, and J. H. Friedman, "Phenomenology of dreams in Parkinson's disease," Movement Disorders, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 198-202, 2007.

[7] P. Bugalho and T. Paiva, "Dream features in the early stages of Parkinson's disease," Journal of Neural Transmission, vol. 118, no. 11, pp. 1613-1619, 2011.

[8] K. Valli, B. Frauscher, T. Peltomaa, V. Gschliesser, A. Revonsuo, and B. Hogl, "Dreaming furiously? A sleep laboratory study on the dream content of people with Parkinson's disease and with or without rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder," Sleep Medicine, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 419-427, 2015.

[9] A. D'Agostino, R. Manni, I. Limosani, M. Terzaghi, S. Cavallotti, and S. Scarone, "Challenging the myth of REM sleep behavior disorder: no evidence of heightened aggressiveness in dreams," Sleep Medicine, vol. 13, no. 6, pp. 714-719, 2012.

[10] N. A. F Frota, E. R. Barbosa, C. S. Porto et al., "Cognitive impairment and magnetic resonance imaging correlations in Wilson's disease," Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, vol. 127, no. 6, pp. 391-398, 2013.

[11] M. G. Carta, L. Saba, M. F. Moro et al., "Homogeneous magnetic resonance imaging of brain abnormalities in bipolar spectrum disorders comorbid with Wilson's disease," General Hospital Psychiatry, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 134-138, 2015.

[12] U. Voss, I. Tuin, K. Schermelleh-Engel, and A. Hobson, "Waking and dreaming: related but structurally independent. Dream reports of congenitally paraplegic and deaf-mute persons," Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 673-687, 2011.

[13] J. H. Friedman, "Dreams in neurological diseases," Medicine and Health, Rhode Island, vol. 89, no. 12, p. 388, 2006.

[14] G. G. Tribl, E. Bor-Seng-Shu, M. C. Trindade, L. T. Lucato, M. J. Teixeira, and E. R. Barbosa, "Wilson's disease presenting as rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: a possible window to early treatment," Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, vol. 72, no. 9, pp. 653-658, 2014.

[15] G. G. Tribl, M. C. Trindade, T. Bittencourt et al., "Wilsons disease with and without rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder compared to healthy matched controls," Sleep Medicine, vol. 17, pp. 179-185, 2015.

[16] M. Solms, The Neuropsychology of Dreams: A Clinico-Anatomical Study, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1997.

[17] C. L. Bassetti, M. Bischof, and P. Valko, "Dreaming: a neurological view," Schweizer Archiv fur Neurologie und Psychiatrie, vol. 156, no. 8, pp. 399-414, 2005.

[18] S. Nevsimalova, J. Buskova, R. Bruha et al., "Sleep disorders in Wilson's disease," European Journal of Neurology, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 184-190, 2011.

[19] K. Portala, K. Westermark, L. Ekselius, and J.-E. Broman, "Sleep in patients with treated Wilson's disease. A questionnaire study," Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 56, no. 4, pp. 291-297, 2002.

[20] European Association for the Study of the Liver, "EASL Clinical Practice Guidelines: Wilson's disease," Journal of Hepatology, vol. 56, no. 3, pp. 671-685, 2012.

[21] B. Frauscher, A. Iranzo, C. Gaig et al., "Normative EMG values during REM sleep for the diagnosis of REM sleep behavior disorder," Sleep, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 835-847, 2012.

[22] V. A. Carvalho, M. T. Barbosa, and P. Caramelli, "Brazilian version of the addenbrooke cognitive examination-revised in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer disease," Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 8-13, 2010.

[23] C. Gorenstein, L. Andrade, A. H. Guerra Vieira Filho, T. C. Tung, and R. Artes, "Psychometric properties of the portuguese version of the Beck Depression Inventory on Brazilian college students," Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 55, no. 5, pp. 553-562, 1999.

[24] A. N. Bertolazi, S. C. Fagondes, L. S. Hoff et al., "Validation of the Brazilian Portuguese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index," Sleep Medicine, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 70-75, 2011.

[25] M. Schredl, R. Binder, S. Feldmann et al., "Dreaming in patients with sleep disorders," Somnologie, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 32-42, 2012.

[26] M. Schredl, "Reliability and stability of a dream recall frequency scale," Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 98, no. 3, pp. 1422-1426, 2004.

[27] M. Schredl, "Effects of state and trait factors on nightmare frequency," European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 253, no. 5, pp. 241-247, 2003.

[28] S. X. Li, Y. K. Wing, S. P. Lam et al., "Validation of a new REM sleep behavior disorder questionnaire (RBDQ-HK)," Sleep Medicine, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 43-48, 2010.

[29] G. W. Domhoff, Finding Meaning in Dreams: A Quantitative Approach, Plenum Press, New York, NY, USA, 1996.

[30] M. Schredl and H. Engelhardt, "Dreaming and psychopathology: dream recall and dream content of psychiatric inpatients," Sleep and Hypnosis, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 44-54, 2001.

[31] M. Schredl, G. Schafer, B. Weber, and I. Heuser, "Dreaming and insomnia: dream recall and dream content of patients with insomnia," Journal of Sleep Research, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 191-198, 1998.

[32] M. Schredl, N. Burchert, and Y. Gabatin, "The effect of training on interrater reliability in dream content analysis," Sleep and Hypnosis, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 139-144, 2004.

[33] L. Murri, R. Massetani, G. Siciliano, L. Giovanditti, and R. Arena, "Dream recall after sleep interruption in brain-injured patients," Sleep, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 356-362,1985.

[34] R. Sandyk, "Weak electromagnetic fields restore dream recall in patients with multiple sclerosis," International Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 82, no. 1-2, pp. 113-125, 1995.

[35] M. Schredl, A. Schroder, and H. Low, "Traumerleben von alteren Menschen--Teil 2: Empirische Studie und Diskussion," Zeitschriftfur Gerontopsychologie und Gerontopsychiatrie, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 43-53, 1996.

[36] P. Brugger, "The phantom limb in dreams," Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 1272-1280, 2008.

[37] R. Bekrater-Bodmann, M. Schredl, M. Diers et al., "Post-amputation pain is associated with the recall of an impaired body representation in dreams--results from a nation-wide survey on limb amputees," PLoS ONE, vol. 10, no. 3, Article ID e0119552, 2015.

[38] E. J. Olson, B. F. Boeve, and M. H. Silber, "Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: demographic, clinical and laboratory findings in 93 cases," Brain, vol. 123, part 2, pp. 331-339, 2000.

[39] C. Scaglione, L. Vignatelli, G. Plazzi et al., "REM sleep behaviour disorder in Parkinson's disease: a questionnaire-based study," Neurological Sciences, vol. 25, no. 6, pp. 316-321, 2005.

[40] A. Bonakis, R. S. Howard, I. O. Ebrahim, S. Merritt, and A. Williams, "REM sleep behaviour disorder (RBD) and its associations in young patients," Sleep Medicine, vol. 10, no. 6, pp. 641-645, 2009.

[41] C. S. Hall and R. L. Van de Castle, The Content Analysis of Dreams, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, NY, USA, 1966.

Gotthard G. Tribl, (1,2) Mateus C. Trindade, (1) Michael Schredl, (3) Joana Pires, (4,5) Iris Reinhard, (6) Thais Bittencourt, (2) Geraldo Lorenzi-Filho, (2) Rosana Cardoso Alves, (1) Daniel Ciampi de Andrade, (1) Erich T. Fonoff, (1) Edson Bor-Seng-Shu, (1) Alexandre A. Machado, (1) Manoel J. Teixeira, (1) and Egberto R. Barbosa (1)

(1) Division of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Hospital das Clinicas, University of Sao Paulo School of Medicine, Avenida Dr. Eneas de Carvalho Aguiar, 255, 5 Andar, Sala 5084, 05403-900 Pinheiros, SP, Brazil

(2) Sleep Laboratory, Pulmonary Division, InCor, University of Sao Paulo School of Medicine, Avenida Dr. Eneas de Carvalho Aguiar 44, 05403-000 Cerqueira Cesar, SP, Brazil

(3) Sleep Laboratory, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, J 5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany

(4) EEG and Sleep Laboratory, Department of Neurosciences, Santa Maria University Hospital, Avenida Prof. Egas Moniz, 1649-035 Lisbon, Portugal

(5) Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology Center (CENC), Rua Conde das Antas no. 5, 1070-068 Lisbon, Portugal

(6) Department of Biostatistics, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, J 5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany

Correspondence should be addressed to Gotthard G. Tribl; gtribl@hotmail.com

Received 10 November 2015; Revised 2 February 2016; Accepted 10 February 2016

Academic Editor: Gianfranco Spalletta
Table 1: Cognitive, affective, and sleep characteristics in
patients with Wilsons disease versus healthy controls and in
WD with RBD versus WD without RBD (mean [+ or -] SD).

                    Wilsons disease      Wilsons           Wilsons
                       (n = 38)       disease without    disease with
                                       RBD (n = 34)       RBD (n = 4)

Female/male              17/21             13/21              4/0
Age [yrs]           34.84 [+ or -]    35.35 [+ or -]    30.50 [+ or -]
                         9.42              9.48              8.81

Severity of         68.82 [+ or -]    69.91 [+ or -]    59.50 [+ or -]
Wilson's disease,        41.12             42.11             34.84
UWDRS [0-320]

Addenbrookes        84.00 [+ or -]    83.53 [+ or -]    88.00 [+ or -]
Cognitive                10.53             10.99             3.83
Examination
[0-100]

ACE-Rl:             16.74 [+ or -]    16.71 [+ or -]    17.00 [+ or -]
attention-               1.75              1.84              0.82
orientation
[0-18]

ACE-R2: memory      20.90 [+ or -]    20.74 [+ or -]    22.25 [+ or -]
[0-26]                   3.85              3.96              2.75

ACE-R3: verbal       8.68 [+ or -]     8.41 [+ or -]    11.00 [+ or -]
fluency [0-14]           3.31              3.33              2.16

ACE-R4: language    24.63 [+ or -]    24.53 [+ or -]    25.50 [+ or -]
[0-26]                   1.82              1.90              0.58

ACE-R5:             13.05 [+ or -]    13.15 [+ or -]    12.25 [+ or -]
visuospatial             2.92              2.95              2.99
abilities [0-16]

Beck Depression     10.66 [+ or -]    10.09 [+ or -]    15.50 [+ or -]
Inventory [0-63]         9.71              9.55              11.21

Pittsburgh Sleep     6.76 [+ or -]     6.47 [+ or -]     9.25 [+ or -]
Quality Index            4.55              4.61              3.59
[0-21]

                       Healthy        All WD    WD + RBD
                       controls       versus    versus
                       (n = 41)        HC p     WD-RBD p

Female/male              17/24          ns       0.019
Age [yrs]           34.32 [+ or -]      ns         ns
                         10.18

Severity of                             --         ns
Wilson's disease,
UWDRS [0-320]

Addenbrookes        91.12 [+ or -]    <0.001       ns
Cognitive                5.52
Examination
[0-100]

ACE-Rl:             17.20 [+ or -]      ns         ns
attention-               1.21
orientation
[0-18]

ACE-R2: memory      22.61 [+ or -]     0.028       ns
[0-26]                   2.93

ACE-R3: verbal      11.37 [+ or -]    <0.001       ns
fluency [0-14]           1.89

ACE-R4: language    24.93 [+ or -]      ns         ns
[0-26]                   1.86

ACE-R5:             15.02 [+ or -]    <0.001       ns
visuospatial             1.24
abilities [0-16]

Beck Depression      5.44 [+ or -]     0.004       ns
Inventory [0-63]         4.28

Pittsburgh Sleep     4.61 [+ or -]     0.013       ns
Quality Index            2.57
[0-21]

ACE-R1-5: Addenbrookes Cognitive Examination subscore from 1
to 5; HC: healthy controls; RBD: REM sleep behaviour
disorder; UWDRS: Unified Wilsons Disease Rating Scale; WD:
Wilsons disease; WD + RBD: Wilsons disease with REM sleep
behaviour disorder; WD--RBD: Wilsons disease without REM
sleep behaviour disorder.

Table 2: Dream and nightmare frequencies and retrospective
dream characteristics (mean [+ or -] SD and percentages).

                   Wilsons disease   Wilsons disease   Wilsons disease
                      (n = 38)         without RBD        with RBD
                                        (n = 34)           (n = 4)

Dream recall        1.84 [+ or -]     1.71 [+ or -]     3.00 [+ or -]
frequency during        2.30              2.27              2.61
last months [n/
week]

Nightmare recall
frequency during    1.18 [+ or -]     0.86 [+ or -]     3.94 [+ or -]
last months [n/         2.33              1.54              5.46
month]

Dream recall        1.32 [+ or -]     1.27 [+ or -]     1.75 [+ or -]
frequency, two/         1.45              1.48              1.19
week diary [n/
week]

RBD                15.54 [+ or -]    12.79 [+ or -]    38.25 [+ or -]
Questionnaire-          12.13             9.01              11.35
Hong Kong, total
score [0-100]

RBDQ-HK1: Did
you often have
dreams?

Lifetime               78.38%            75.76%            100.00%
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year       2.32 [+ or -]     2.18 [+ or -]     3.50 [+ or -]
frequency [0-4]         1.44              1.45              0.58

RBDQ-HK2: Did
you often have
nightmares ?

Lifetime               59.46%            54.55%            100.00%
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year       0.73 [+ or -]     0.49 [+ or -]     2.75 [+ or -]
frequency [0-4]         0.99              0.71              0.50

RBDQ-HK3: Did
you have dreams
with an
emotional and
sorrowful
content?

Lifetime               70.27%            66.67%            100.00%
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year       0.92 [+ or -]     0.82 [+ or -]     1.75 [+ or -]
frequency [0-4]         1.04              0.92              1.71

RBDQ-HK4: Did
you have dreams
with a violent
or aggressive
content (e.g,
fighting with
someone)?

Lifetime               43.24%            39.39%            75.00%
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year       0.43 [+ or -]     0.33 [+ or -]     1.25 [+ or -]
frequency [0-4]         0.80              0.69              1.26

RBDQ-HK5: Did
you have dreams
with a
frightening and
horrifying
content (e.g,
being chased by
a ghost)?

Lifetime               37.84%            33.33%            75.00%
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year       0.43 [+ or -]     0.30 [+ or -]     1.50 [+ or -]
frequency [0-4]         0.77              0.59              1.29

                      Healthy       All WD    WD + RBD
                     controls       versus     versus
                      (n = 41)       HC p     WD-RBD p

Dream recall       1.95 [+ or -]      ns         ns
frequency during        2.06
last months [n/
week]

Nightmare recall
frequency during   0.65 [+ or -]      ns       0.040
last months [n/         1.03
month]

Dream recall       1.48 [+ or -]      ns         ns
frequency, two/         1.41
week diary [n/
week]

RBD                13.71 [+ or -]     ns       <0.001
Questionnaire-          5.67
Hong Kong, total
score [0-100]

RBDQ-HK1: Did
you often have
dreams?

Lifetime               92.68%         ns         ns
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year      2.78 [+ or -]      ns         ns
frequency [0-4]         1.11

RBDQ-HK2: Did
you often have
nightmares ?

Lifetime               68.29%         ns         ns
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year      0.73 [+ or -]      ns       0.001
frequency [0-4]         0.81

RBDQ-HK3: Did
you have dreams
with an
emotional and
sorrowful
content?

Lifetime               75.61%         ns         ns
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year      1.00 [+ or -]      ns         ns
frequency [0-4]         0.84

RBDQ-HK4: Did
you have dreams
with a violent
or aggressive
content (e.g,
fighting with
someone)?

Lifetime               68.29%        0.026       ns
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year      0.51 [+ or -]      ns       0.028
frequency [0-4]         0.64

RBDQ-HK5: Did
you have dreams
with a
frightening and
horrifying
content (e.g,
being chased by
a ghost)?

Lifetime               65.85%       0.013        ns
occurrence [y/n]

Recent 1-year      0.29 [+ or -]    ns         0.051
frequency [0-4]         0.46

HC: healthy controls; RBD: REM sleep behaviour disorder;
RBD-HK1-5: RBD Questionnaire-Hong Kong questions 1 to 5; WD:
Wilsons disease; WD + RBD: Wilsons disease with REM sleep
behaviour disorder; WD--RBD: Wilsons disease without REM
sleep behaviour disorder.

Table 3: Self-rated dream emotions and dream content
analysis by external judge; Wilson's disease versus healthy
controls (mean [+ or -] SD and percentages).

                                                           All WD
                            Wilson's         Healthy       versus
                           disease          controls        HC p
                            (n = 26)         (n = 35)

Female/male                  12/14            15/20          ns

Dream reports per        4.31 [+ or -]    4.17 [+ or -]      ns
participant [n]               2.06             2.26

Word count per dream     74.52 [+ or -]   98.30 [+ or -]   0.006
report [n]                   69.27            61.41

Bizarreness [0-3]        0.65 [+ or -]    0.91 [+ or -]    ns (1)
                              0.98             1.10

Positive emotions        1.31 [+ or -]    1.48 [+ or -]    ns (1)
(self-rating) [0-3]           1.26             1.09

Negative emotions        1.21 [+ or -]    1.31 [+ or -]    ns (1)
(self-rating) [0-3]           1.27             1.06

Positive emotions        0.49 [+ or -]    0.51 [+ or -]    -- (2)
(external judge) [0-3]        0.89             0.90

Negative emotions        0.73 [+ or -]    0.70 [+ or -]    ns (1)
(external judge) [0-3]        1.07             1.03

Number of characters     1.55 [+ or -]    2.03 [+ or -]    ns (1)
[n]                           1.44             1.60

Number of male           0.59 [+ or -]    0.81 [+ or -]    ns (1)
persons [n]                   0.89             1.01

Number of female         0.71 [+ or -]    0.64 [+ or -]    ns (1)
persons [n]                   0.82             0.86

Verbal interaction           49.11%           52.74%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Physical interaction         14.29%           21.92%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Health-related               5.36%            2.74%        ns (1)
topics [y/n]

Professional                 7.14%            10.96%       ns (1)
environment [y/n]

Leisure [y/n]                48.21%           54.79%       ns (1)

Problems [0-2]           0.41 [+ or -]    0.53 [+ or -]    ns (1)
                              0.64             0.68

Movements of legs            25.00%           35.62%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Movements of arms            18.75%           26.71%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Reference to legs            5.36%            2.74%        ns (1)
or arms [y/n]

Physical activity            15.18%           14.38%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Restriction of               1.79%            3.42%        ns (1)
movements [y/n]

Verbal and physical          9.82%            8.22%        ns (1)
aggression towards
dream ego [y/n]

Aggression, total            15.18%           10.96%       ns (1)
[y/n]

Notes. (1) Group effect, word count was included as
covariate. (2) Algorithm did not converge. HC: healthy
controls; WD: Wilson's disease.

Table 4: Dream content analysis of aggressive themes; WD
with RBD versus WD without RBD (mean [+ or -] SD and percentages).

                      Wilson's         Wilson's       WD + RBD   Effect
                    disease with    disease without    versus     size
                    RBD (n = 4)      RBD (n = 22)     WD-RBD p

Female/male             4/0              8/14           0.19     0.976

Dream reports      4.50 [+ or -]     4.27 [+ or -]       ns      0.098
per patient [n]         2.65             2.00

Word count per     69.90 [+ or -]   66.83 [+ or -]       ns      0.080
dream report [n]       31.47             43.87

Positive           0.98 [+ or -]     1.51 [+ or -]       ns      -0.570
emotions (self-         0.90             0.96
rating) [0-3]

Negative           2.58 [+ or -]     1.03 [+ or -]     0.031     1.915
emotions (self-         0.52             1.02
rating) [0-3]

Positive           0.48 [+ or -]     0.53 [+ or -]       ns      -0.090
emotions                0.36             0.70
(external judge)
[0-3]

Negative           1.25 [+ or -]     0.58 [+ or -]     0.081     1.055
emotions                0.64             0.63
(external judge)
[0-3]

Dreams with        0.50 [+ or -]     0.27 [+ or -]       ns      0.406
verbal                  0.58             0.55
aggression
towards others
[n]

Dreams with        1.00 [+ or -]     0.05 [+ or -]     0.021     1.587
verbal                  0.82             0.21
aggression
towards dream
ego [n]

Dreams with              0           0.05 [+ or -]       ns      -0.337
physical                                 0.21
aggression
towards others
[n]

Dreams with        0.75 [+ or -]     0.18 [+ or -]       ns      0.519
physical                1.50             0.40
aggression
towards dream
ego [n]

Dreams with        1.50 [+ or -]     0.23 [+ or -]     0.048     1.321
verbal and              1.29             0.43
physical
aggression
towards dream
ego [n]

Dreams with        1.75 [+ or -]     0.46 [+ or -]     0.016     1.558
aggression,             0.96             0.67
total [n]

WD + RBD: Wilsons disease with REM sleep behaviour disorder;
WD-RBD: Wilsons disease without REM sleep behaviour
disorder.
COPYRIGHT 2016 Hindawi Limited
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2016 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:Research Article
Author:Tribl, Gotthard G.; Trindade, Mateus C.; Schredl, Michael; Pires, Joana; Reinhard, Iris; Bittencourt
Publication:Behavioural Neurology
Article Type:Report
Date:Jan 1, 2016
Words:7486
Previous Article:Neuroprotective effects of bone marrow mesenchymal stem cells on bilateral common carotid arteries occlusion model of cerebral ischemia in rat.
Next Article:The intimate relationship between vestibular migraine and Meniere disease: a review of pathogenesis and presentation.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2019 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters