Printer Friendly

Dissecting learning in Aplysia.

Dissecting learning in Aplysia

To understand how a machine works, an engineer might take it apart. One way for a neurobiologist to discover the mechanisms underlying various forms of learning is to trace how these are put together in the nervous system as a young animal matures. Thomas J. Carew at Yale University and his co-workers have applied this developmental approach to the study of nonassociative learning in Aplysia, a marine snail whose adult form became famous in pioneering studies of the molecular basis of memory (SN: 1/22/83, p.58).

Carew's group looked for the onset of three kinds of learning --habituation, dishabituation and sensitization--by testing how readily Aplysia contracts its siphon when this organ is sprayed with a jet of water. After a series of such stimuli, the snail habituates, ignoring the water jets. If the researchers then shock Aplysia's tail, it again becomes responsive to water jets. This is dishabituation. Sensitization occurs when a unstimulated animal first gets a tail shock, prompting its siphon to contract even more readily than usual.

Conventional thinking has held that sensitization and dishabituation are part of the same process. But Carew has now shown that the two are separate because they emerge at different times in development. Moreover, he discovered a fourth kind of learning, called inhibition, which had not been seen in adults until now because it is masked by other processes.

The researchers are now working to uncover the cellular and neural mechanisms that are "turned on' when Aplysia begins to show each kind of learning. Carew notes that the beginning of sensitization is accompanied by an eight-fold increase in the number of cells in the nervous system. "Either there's new circuitry being added [for sensitization],' he says, "and/or the signal that's [triggering the proliferation of new cells] is also turning on sensitization.'
COPYRIGHT 1987 Science Service, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1987, Gale Group. All rights reserved. Gale Group is a Thomson Corporation Company.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:marine snail
Author:Weisburd, Stefi
Publication:Science News
Date:Oct 31, 1987
Words:306
Previous Article:Piano playing preserved in dementia.
Next Article:Hidden histories on death row.
Topics:


Related Articles
Simple 'learning' in cell culture.
New connections may be memorable.
Memories might be made of this: closing in on the biochemistry of learning.
Evaluation of the Population Status of the Tumbling Creek Cavesnail (Hydrobiidae: Antrobia culveri). (Speleology Section).
Salt marsh snails plow leaves, fertilize fungus.
Plicopurpura pansa (Gould, 1853) from the Pacific Coast of Mexico and Central America: a traditional source of Tyrian Purple.
Variations in density, shell-size and growth with shore height and wave exposure of the rocky intertidal snail, Calyptraea spirata (Forbes, 1852), in...
Reproductive biology of the "Copey" snail Melongena melongena (Linnaeus, 1758) in Cispata Bay on the Caribbean coast of Colombia.
Biological and chemical properties of the secretion from the hypobranchial gland of the purple snail Plicopurpura pansa (Gould, 1853).
Gastropod abundance and biomass relationships with salt marsh vegetation within ocean-dominated South Carolina, USA estuaries.

Terms of use | Copyright © 2016 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters