Printer Friendly

Calculating apes: when the chips are down, do chimpanzees sum quantities in a simple way linked to the human capacity for counting?

Calculating Apes

Sherman and Austin, two chimpanzeeshoused at the Yerkes Regional Primate Research Center in Atlanta, are fond of chocolate. Their sweet tooth, however, has led psychologists at nearby Georgia State University to a distinctly noncaloric conclusion: Sherman and Austin, when presented with two trays of chocolates, can perform a basic type of calculation that may be a precursor of more advanced arithmetic skills used by humans.

"As far as we know, these chimps can'tcount and do not know numbers,' says psychologist Duane M. Rumbaugh. "What's important is that they can somehow combine separate piles of chocolates and, without any reinforcement other than the immediate food reward, choose the pair that nets them the greater amount.'

Rumbaugh and colleagues Sue Savage-Rumbaughand Mark T. Hegel say that this mental operation is a form of "summation.' The exact way in which summation works is not clear, but the process involves joining pairs of separate quantities and determining which combination contains the most items. Rumbaugh notes that this is not addition, in which numbers that represent totals of separate sets of items are combined. Addition and other counting operations rely on several abilities, including the tagging of individual items with an ordered series of numbers and knowing that the number assigned to the last member of a counted set also represents the total number of items in the set.

Although research on language abilitiesof apes and controversy over the findings continue, increasing attention is being focused on whether they can carry out primitive types of calculation that might be linked to the human ability to count.

Sherman and Austin were allowed tochoose between two sliding trays placed against a chain link fence enclosing their exercise yard. Each tray contained two food wells with varying amounts of chocolate chips. They could poke a finger through the fence to obtain the chocolate, but once contact with a tray was made, the other tray was immediately drawn out of reach by an experimenter. Each chimp made this type of choice 50 times a day for six days.

In experiments where each food wellcontained from zero to four chocolate chips, the chimpanzees chose the tray with the greater sum more than 90 percent of the time. For example, a tray with three chips in one well and three chips in the other well was nearly always picked over a tray with four chips in one well and none in the remaining well. When the maximum number of chocolates in a food well was increased to five, Sherman and Austin still chose the tray with the greater sum more than 90 percent of the time.

Their most difficult task was distinguishingbetween sums of seven and eight. In one such case, a tray with five chips in one well and two chips in the other was placed next to a tray with four chips in each well. The chimps chose the larger of these sums 79 percent of the time.

In the April JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTALPSYCHOLOGY: ANIMAL BEHAVIOR PROCESSES, the researchers offer a preliminary explanation of Sherman and Austin's successes. The chimps may first have "subitized' the number of chocolates in each food well. Subitizing--perceiving at a glance up to five items without actually counting them--has been observed in human children and adults. How it works, and its role, if any, in the emergence of counting skills is unclear. Each pair of subitized amounts could have been combined or summated to obtain an estimate of the tray totals, with the summations then compared before a choice was made.

"This is only a model,' says Rumbaugh,"but what we've observed with Sherman and Austin seems to be a rudimentary calculation system. They weren't counting, but they got beyond the limits of subitizing.'

As in ape language studies, however,the significance of the chimps' performance is open to interpretation.

Emil W. Menzel of the State Universityof New York at Stony Brook has only one objection to the model proposed by the Georgia investigators. "Calling the chimps' calculation system "rudimentary' is putting them down,' he says. "This study importantly extends previous work on ape calculation.'

In 1960, Menzel reported that chimpsquickly learned to distinguish between two colored plaques, one of which resulted in a larger food reward. The apes also learned to rank five opaque plaques according to the amount of food they covered.

Sherman and Austin's chocolate chipchoices are an important first step in studying the ability of animals to perform operations on numbers, says Hank Davis of Guelph (Canada) University, but summation remains a murky concept. "Summation may not be a precursor to addition,' he maintains. "I'm not convinced that there is a developmental link between the two abilities.'

Davis and his colleagues have foundthat in some situations rats can discriminate between small numbers of events. For example, when three mild shocks are administered during an experimental session, the animals are less apt to press a lever previously associated with food, but they readily learn to resume lever pressing after the third and final shock. When the situation is made more complex by introducing another cue, such as a tone before and after each shock, lever pressing does not rebound after the third shock.

These discriminations are more refinedthan judgments of "several' or "few,' says Davis, but there is no solid evidence that rats, chimps or any other animals can count.

Another important study of what maybe calculation without counting by an ape was reported in the May 2, 1985, NATURE. Tetsuro Matsuzawa of Kyoto (Japan) University trained a chimp to name 14 objects and 11 colors by choosing among a set of symbols. The chimp then learned to select from a keyboard an Arabic numeral, from one to six, matching the number of objects displayed. When, for example, five blue toothbrushes were shown, the animal pressed keys bearing "5' and symbols for "blue' and "toothbrush.'

In this case, says Davis, although thechimp often tagged objects with an appropriate number, it may have formed associations with a "jumble of unrelated number tags' rather than demonstrating knowledge of an ordered series of numbers beginning with "1' and ending with "6.'

Unlike Davis and Menzel, David Premackof the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia says the chocolate chip experiment with Sherman and Austin does not add up to much. "Summation has to do with quantity judgment, not counting,' says Premack, who is also Matsuzawa's postdoctoral supervisor. "The discrimination [that the Georgia researchers] are looking at is so primitive, I'd be surprised if a housefly couldn't do it.'

In his own experiments with a language-trainedchimp named Sarah, Premack has found that she can match like proportions of objects that do not look alike. For instance, given a choice between a glass of water one-quarter full and three-quarters full, Sarah correctly matches the latter item with three separated quarters off an apple.

Premack suggests that the Georgiaresearchers must establish whether their chimps can distinguish between, say, three small chocolate chips and two larger chips that take up the same amount of space.

Rumbaugh acknowledges that Shermanand Austin may have combined what they saw as unitary amounts of food, not subitized values, based on estimates of the surface area of each pile of chips. He and his colleagues, however, have found that another language-trained chimp, Lana, appears to be able to count up to three items on a computer screen by using a joystick to control a cursor. Since the size of each item and the volume of quantities can be varied on a computer, the researchers plan to conduct further summation experiments with this technology.

Results so far with Lana are "exciting,'says Rumbaugh, and indicate that she is focusing on quantity, not volume. It would be interesting, he adds, to give summation tests to chimps with no language training. Premack holds that, although he believes no important calculation abilities were uncovered in the chocolate chip experiment, language training boosts a chimp's analytical reasoning skills and ability to grasp small numbers and simple fractions (SN: 12/5/81, p.363). Davis, on the other hand, says that language is not a requirement to use numbers or judge quantities, since human infants show "remarkable numerical ability.' In one study, 7-month-olds showed a preference for looking at an array of objects that matched in number a sequence of sounds presented by experimenters.

"True' counting by children beginsat around 4 to 5 years of age, but appears to develop in stages, says Brendan McGonigle of the University of Edinburgh, Scotland. For instance, many 6-year-olds cannot count above 10. At first, the number "1' is often used as an anchor to begin counting; performance falters if counting starts from a higher number. Furthermore, forward counting is mastered before counting backwards.

Summation may be a primitive link inthe developmental chain of counting skills, says Rumbaugh. "It may reflect something for which chimpanzees have a need in the wild,' he suggests. One possibility is that chimps use a "natural, number-related response' to decide which cluster of berries on a bush should be approached and which branch bears the greater number of buds.

"Studies of this type of behavior in thewild would be fascinating,' says Rumbaugh. "But the key question is, when does formal counting become a requisite for these kinds of judgments?'

Photo: Sherman and Austin, who often choose the greater of two paired amounts of chocolate chips, huddle together in a language-training laboratory.

Photo: Behind Shermanand Austin is a keyboard with geometric symbols that they were trained to communicate with, but did not use in the chocolate chip "summation' experiment.
COPYRIGHT 1987 Science Service, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1987, Gale Group. All rights reserved. Gale Group is a Thomson Corporation Company.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Bower, Bruce
Publication:Science News
Date:May 23, 1987
Words:1590
Previous Article:AIDS vaccines: the problems of human testing.
Next Article:Early human skeleton apes its ancestors.
Topics:


Related Articles
Denker done as South's girls basketball coach.
Arson attacks ruled terrorism.
Genesis meets the Big Bang and evolution, absent design.
Darwin and democracy.
How will it all end? Eschatology in science and religion.
Brains, bodies, beliefs, and behavior.
Teaching science and religion in a Jewish seminary.
Self-sacrificial love: evolutionary deception or theological reality?
"Intelligent design," Natural Design, and the problem of meaning in the natural world.

Terms of use | Copyright © 2017 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters