Printer Friendly

Brilliance as cognitive complexity in Aboriginal Australia.

ABSTRACT

Writers on Australian Aboriginal ceremonial practices and contemporary commercial art have identified properties that can be described as producing 'brilliance', the term used by Howard Morphy (1989) for Yolngu visual art. Brilliance involves the emotionally charged knowledge that Ancestral power is manifest. We suggest that, in Australian Aboriginal ceremonial practices and art, two kinds of brilliance are at work, both of which we exemplify using evidence from songs and languages of the Arandic and Warlpiri region of Central Australia. One kind of brilliance is manifest as visual brightness and whiteness, the other relates to a variety of cognitive complexities that involve diverse combinations of parts both within and across modalities of expression. In extending the notion of brilliance from the visuality of brightness to include cognitive complexity, we follow other work on Australian Aboriginal ceremonial practices. Seeking to understand why these properties should produce particular emotional and epistemic effects in participants, we draw on psychological accounts of the sublime and awe (Keltner and Haidt 2003; Miall and Kuiken 1994), which we treat as part of the same broad family of experiences as the experience of brilliance. We suggest both visual brightness and cognitive complexity represent an 'extremity' that as such places considerable cognitive demands on the participants. These extraordinary cognitive demands stimulate emotion and produce the experience of Ancestral power and surprising moments of insight.

Keywords: Aboriginal Australia, aesthetics, awe, brilliance, ceremony, cognition, sublime.

BRILLIANCE

Morphy on bir'yun or brilliance in Yolngu bark painting

According to Howard Morphy (1989: 28), '[i]t is the quality of brilliance that is associated in Yolngu art with Ancestral power and with beauty'. Following field notes by Donald Thomson, he translates the Yolngu term bir'yun as 'brilliance' and explains it to be a 'sensation of light, the uplift of looking at this carefully carried out work. They see in it the likeness to the wan-garr [Ancestral past] (Morphy 1989: 28, citing Thomson's unpublished field notes). The emotional effects of brilliance include joy and happiness, which arise from the 'shimmering quality of light'. The bark is painted red and over this background, figurative and geometric components are added in yellow or black ochre, then cross-hatched white lines are added, at which point the painting acquires brilliance. As Moiphy (1989:33) goes on to explain:

The technique of cross-hatching results in a painting whose surface shimmers with light and which appears full of movement and life. This brilliance represents the Ancestral power of the painting shining from within.

'Brilliance' has been associated in other parts of Aboriginal Australia with Ancestral power.

We suggest that brilliance comprises two distinguishable properties. The first is primarily perceptual and involves whiteness, brightness, and shine, sometimes with movement such as shimmering and flickering; often this property is transient. The second property arises from the fact that ceremonial practices of song, dance, and painting often involve the complex combination of simple component parts. These component parts are combined not only sequentially but also simultaneously, such that they can be experienced at the same time, acoustically in the rhythm, melody, or text of the singing or percussive accompaniment; and visually in the designs on canvas or the body and movements of the dancers. We suggest that this simultaneity is a second property of brilliance.

Morphy describes brilliance as having an emotional effect accompanied by a belief 'in the magical properties or the spiritual power, or the effect of a particular object' (Morphy 2009:6). Ancestors are always present in the invisible realm of the 'Dreaming', what Stanner (1956) called the 'Everywhen' and which we refer to throughout as Altyerre, as it is known in Arrernte and Kaytetye. But the Ancestors emerge into the realm of the visible because the communication channels are opened. We suggest that though only the participants experience Ancestral truths, these truths are nonetheless produced by general psychological mechanisms; in this our approach is similar to that of William James (1902) who thought of mystical experience as access to a deeper reality via the special operation of general psychological mechanisms. Central to our argument is that we associate participants' knowledge of Ancestral power with their skill and experience, which provides them with the necessary knowledge that the interconnected component parts of the ceremony have been performed correctly. (The term 'participant' Includes Ingram's (2013) notion of a 'listening participant', which can be divided into different roles; e.g., someone being painted-up has a different role to the lead singer, yet both are participants.)

We do not have direct evidence of what participants' experience is. Where we quote expressions of emotional impact, they are almost always from Turpin's ethnographic observations during performances and statements by other fieldworkers rather than participants themselves. We assume that complex ritual performance practices are sustained because they satisfy the cognitive, emotional, and epistemic needs of the participants. The problem of introspection is the same in work on aesthetic practices (including ritual practices) in other cultures, since explicit introspection is unreliable (Nisbett and Wilson 1977).

In this paper, we draw on Myfany Turpin's fieldwork on Arandic songs set into the wider context of the ethnographic research of Aboriginal religion and art. While we do not draw specifically on T. G. H. Strelow's monumental work in this area (which consists almost entirely of restricted Arandic men's songs of which we have no first-hand experience), we are in keeping with Strehlow's demonstration that songs are high culture comparable to the musical achievements in other cultural traditions. Most relevant for our discussion is that songs are cognitively demanding. While each element of song on its own is often simple and repetitive (e.g., text, rhythm, melody, and design) the creation and processing of the thick layering of multiple auditory and visual components demands significant cognitive work. We argue that this cognitive work is one of the types of brilliance that produces the effect of Ancestral power.

BRIGHTNESS AND TRANSIENCE

Brilliance as brightness, whiteness, and transience

Yolngu brilliance includes brightness and transience, and these are found across Aboriginal Australia in body painting, ground art, sand story narratives, song, dance, and the overall

effect of ceremonies. Brightness effects involve whiteness, shininess, shimmering, glittering, and iridescence. These visual effects arise also from oil gleaming on the skin, flickering fire, rays of the sun, mirages, and surface water effects. Brilliance can exist in how the world is perceived and understood, as well as in ceremony. Transience is manifested in practices of momentary looking or touching, in the concealing and revealing of sacra, in the erasure of marks on the ceremonial ground, and in the perceived changing colours of iridescent objects such as pearl shell.

Morphy and John Carty (2015:110) describe 'shimmer' in the acrylic dots of desert paintings, and compare these dots to ground sculpture and the balls of 'bush cotton' which are attached to men's bodies and shaken off in performance, producing 'traces of ancestral power'. Franchise Dussart (1988:37) discusses shininess in Warlpiri painting, which 'recalls the Ancestral Beings completely beautified when they originally emerged from the Ancestral ground'. Jennifer Deger (2006:190) says of a Yolngu-made film that 'these shots of water shimmer, deriving a power from something beyond representation. They are, and effect, a brilliance of surface and depth, reflection and illumination'. Iridescent objects such as pearl shells, which were used as ceremonial exchange items and as objects of beauty, were traded over a 1000 kilometres, which suggests the high esteem in which these were and are held (Akerman and Stanton, 1994). In Arandic languages, pearl shell is referred to with the same words used to describe the property of stars, the sun, glass, silver objects, and reflecting water (e.g., alim-alime).(1) Diana Young (2006:240; 2011:367) says that people on the Anangu Pitjantjatjara Yankunytjatjara Lands in northern South Australia find evidence of Ancestral power in the capacity of the land and sky to change colour and to fleetingly become highly visible and then again invisible at sunset.

That brilliance involves both whiteness and shininess has been observed for a number of Aboriginal cultures. Amongst the Anbarra of Arnhemland, colour is a binary division glossed broadly as 'light' and 'dark', whereby light requires 'a touch of brilliance or "animation" as well as a high degree of brightness' (Jones and Meehan 1978:27). A similar binary grouping exists for Anindilyakwa speakers on Groote Eylandt, which Velma Leeding (1989) refers to as a division into 'lustre' and 'lustreless'. Brilliant objects used in Central Australian ceremony include white headdresses and white headbands traditionally made from hair string and rubbed with white ochre, replaced now with white cloth or fabric cut into lengths (Fig. 1, Warlpiri dancers). White natural objects are also used, including cockatoo feathers, 'bush cotton' (white down or crushed flowers), white ochre, and white tail tips of the bilby (or rabbit). Warlpiri performers wear what they refer to as 'white' (pale yellow) and red necklaces made from the seeds of the bat-wing coral tree (Erythrina vespertilio) (Fig. 1, Warlpiri dancers). (2)

These everyday objects can be transformed into ceremonial objects when placed on people or sacra and 'sung'. Even a modern shiny object such as a belt buckle can be sung and thus transformed into a ceremonial and power-laden object. Visual brilliance appears on the performers' bodies, which glisten with oil and ritual designs that can include white, yellow, and red ochres as well as black obtained from charcoal. In women's ceremonies, white is the final colour to be applied to the body and performers say that this makes the design as a whole stand out (Turpin 2015:79). Yolngu artists give the same explanation for the final outlining of body designs in white (Morphy 2016, personal communication).

Visual brilliance can be seen in clouds, sun, stars, mirages, water, limestone, mica, butterflies, white ochre, flowers, feathers, glistening sunlight, sap and fat, as well as glass, mirrors, and various metals. Verbs in Arandic languages that describe the potential 'shimmering' properties of such objects are also used to name representations of shimmer in art and dance, thus drawing an analogy between ritual practice and properties of the world: the dancers' movements 'shimmer' and paintings 'glisten' as do glass and water. Such verbs

include erlkwerlke- 'flash, blink, glisten', ile- 'shine, flash', arrke-, alharrke- 'shine, flash', amparrke-, amperrkarre- 'shine, sparkle', and arraly-arralyarre- 'twinkle, glitter' (cf., Turpin 2015).

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

In ceremonial dance brilliance is produced by movement. Moreover, Peter Sutton and Michael Snow (2015:96) suggest that movement is a perceptual property of stationary brilliant objects with structural colour (as when colour is produced not by pigmentation but by the layered structure of the object) even if they do not actually move: 'The iridescent has more power than the merely shiny because it does more visual work. It is the still, bright, dense thing that moves'. Dancing involves regular movement to the musical beat and ritual objects are synchronized in the hands of the dancers whose thudding feet and breasts echo the musical beat. Joseph Jordania (2011) associates such entrainment with solidarity and trance, while Judith Becker (1994: 50) sees that it 'invokes realms of knowledge to which we otherwise have little access'. In Aboriginal ceremonies, synchronized rhythmic dancing is often juxtaposed with a fast movement where thighs and ceremonial leaves are shaken and feathered headdresses and armlets quiver (Fig. 1, Warlpiri dancers). Thighs were once oiled and painted, making the brilliant effect more salient. Rapid movement of leaves, clap-sticks, and boomerangs produces tremolo, and perhaps this is an aural equivalent of visual brilliance.

The representation of brilliance

Brilliance and whiteness as well as objects with these qualities are also the subjects of songs. Women's awelye songs include references to white ochre (1), as well as white ritual items such as the ritual pole (2), ritual headband (3, 6) and forehead adornment (4), and headdress (5). These are exemplified below with verses (which when repeated form a song). (3)
(1.)
Ngwenty-ngwentyelarl  With white ochre,
areyep arrernelhek    they adorned themselves
Aneyarr-therrel       The two young women,
areyep arrernelhek    adorned themselves
                      (w_Arnka070404_02, Alyawarr)

(2.)
Mangaya kurrkurla     The (white) feathers appear shining on the
jingka-jingka-wangka

Wirnparrku kurrkurla  ceremonial pole
jingka-jingka-wangka  The feathers appear shining at Wirnparrku
                      (Gallagher et al. 2014:20, Warlpiri)

(3.)
Merrpewantyerraye           Tie it tight
Iterlerre merrpere arrerne  Tie the headband tight
                            (Turpin 2005, appendix 1)

(4.)
Alpitarle atyengarle
ake arrkerl-aneke       The forehead decoration on my head shimmers
Akerlkentye atyengarle
akerlkerl-aneke         My white adornment is shining
                        (JG02_020328-08, Arrernte)


The language of brightness can be complex and allusive. In (4) the verb akerlkentye 'head shine-NMZ' is literally 'that which shines or blossoms white'. In Alyawarr the verb can also mean 'covered in ash', 'sparkle'. In (5) the word for the white feathered headdress on a ceremonial pole (Fig. 2), karlkwerrarlkerre, is a compound of ake 'head' plus a nominalized form of the verb arlkw- 'to eat', which is used to mean 'out do', and thus a translation of this word could be 'the best crown of them all'.
(5.)
Karlkwarrarlkwarrarl rtnenhek  Where the spectacular headress stood
Irrtyarew renhap
tyarewetyarel rnkernek         On the distant ceremonial pole standing
                               (Turpin et al in press, Alyawarr)


Often polysemy is based on a shared bright, white colour. For example, the Alyawarr word for 'white headband', tyerlarr, is replaced with a poetic word, iylerlkarr in (6). This word appears to be related to the pan-Arandic word iylerlkenng 'Sandover lily (Crinum flaccidum)', a rare plant characterized by its large white flower, which is supported by an unelicited observation by Arrernte singer M.K. Turner (25 May 2017, personal communication). (4) In the dance accompanying this couplet, the headband is held up in the air and moved from side to side in a characteristically brilliant combination of whiteness and movement (see Fig. 3).
(6.)
Alker ilewerr-ilewerrirrenty      The shimmering horizon
Iylerlkarrelarl alamperrkelhelek  The headband made it glisten
                                  (Turpin and Ross 2013:32, Alyawarr)


In addition to words for white ceremonial items, the verses often contain words that refer to their brilliant visual effect, as seen in examples (2), (4), and (6). Words for 'glimpse', 'show', 'reveal,' and 'see' are also common (7), perhaps highlighting the transient or fleeting aspect of brilliance.
(7.)
Arnkely-arnkelyelarl lwewarrernek  The grumpy woman revealed (them)
Arlkeny malangkarl lwewarrernek    Revealed the lovely
                                   ceremonial designs
                                   (Turpin and Ross
                                   2013:45, Alyawarr)


[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

Some verses refer to yellow ochre (Gallagher et al. 2014) and yellow flowers, a colour that also has a brightness to it. There appear to be no verses that refer to red or black objects: we do not know whether this gap is unique to women's songs or to all ceremonial songs. There are a number of brilliant objects referred to frequently in song texts that are not used in ceremony, such as butterflies, clouds, and mirages.

[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]

If a verse refers to brightness, there are usually accompanying actions (not all verses are about bright objects), and conversely where there are no accompanying actions, verses do not generally mention brightness or brilliant objects. This points again to the significance of the intimate connection between brightness and simultaneity, which we have suggested are two aspects of 'brilliance'.

Brightness and power in other cultures

Brilliance is a property associated with power in the aesthetic practices of other cultures too, for example Abelam painting, about which Morphy (2005) notes that the geometric components and cross-hatching resemble those in Yolngu art. Brigitta Hauser-Schaublin (1996:87) writes that in Abelam art, the most valued colour is white, and the white line the most important line. Paul Roscoe (1995:10) ties Abelam brilliance to the feeling of power: 'The ability of the Ka nimbia's bright colours to arrest the attention, then, was experienced as a sensation of alien power and danger, a feeling of being in the presence of a powerful and menacing entity' (1995:12). Eric Kjellgren (2007:62) describes Abelam art as having 'a magnificence that overwhelms the viewer with the complexity, scale, and brilliant colours of the sacred display', and describes the response as 'awe' (which we discuss further below).

Also in Papua New Guinea, Douglas Dalton (1996) describes how for the Rawa of Morobe Province, shell-and-tooth-based valuable ornaments called kunawo 'make the eye rejoice', and that the men ritually cook the shells over a fire with lime powder and perform a magical incantation to make them bright and shiny 'like the sun'. Deborah Neff (1987: 67) observes for South Indian Keralan ground painting that 'the more eye-dazzling the floor drawing, the more powerful an object of concentration and invocation it is'. Turning to European art, Morphy compares Yolngu brilliance to the 'luminosity' of Vermeer's paintings. Studies of European art and architecture have emphasised how light, brilliance, and magnificence may have an effect associated with profound religious experience. For the great 16th century, Italian architect Andrea Palladio 'white was the colour most pleasing to God' (Anderson 2013:46).

Critics have identified brilliance in mediaeval European religious art. For instance, Charles Dodwell (1993:36, 376) discusses the interest in iridescence and brightness, and Christine Hunzinger (2015) shows that mediaeval European objects that have brilliance (dazzle, flash, sparkle, and scintillation), multiplicity, and visual complexity are 'marvellous' and produce wonder. Stephen Jaeger (2010) discusses how meditation on brightness, including colour and light signifying the unrepresentable, can lead 'the dull mind to truth', and Patricia Miller (2000:234) discusses dazzle, glitter, and flash in early Christian mosaics and other art 'in which the boundary between body and spirit, death and life, is dissolved in a shimmer of light'.

These examples, chosen from across time and cultures, raise the possibility that there is some psychologically generalizable property of bright, white or shiny objects, and paintings that produces a generally valued kind of experience, across cultures--an experience associated with spiritual power and awe, and widely documented for the dying, near-death experience.

Brilliance and perceptual difficulty

Brilliance may make it harder to perceive the form of the image, song, dance, or other element. For example, the white crosshatching on the surface of Yolngu paintings makes the underlying symbolic representations harder to see, at least for the untrained eye. Sutton and Snow (2015:138, 134-5) write of acrylic dot painting that 'the dotting tends to set up a shimmering in the eye', and that these are interference effects making it harder to see the objects. Fast performance of a song may make the song harder to hear. Perception can be hindered by brilliance-associated phenomena such as a mirage, rain falling, or the dust rising from the ground. In an account of general musical listening, David Huron (2006:324) suggests that musical dissonance is similar to visual glare because both interfere with our perception of the form. In this regard, we note that group singing of Australian songs does not require the perfect matching of pitch, and instead there may be a heterophony or slight musical dissonance. Additionally, songs are sometimes performed by several performers out of phase, making the text difficult to hear (Hale 1984:257). Alternations to the sounds and even morphology of words are found in many song traditions, along with performance practices that make the words difficult to perceive as distinct words with specific meanings. These include the practice of starting a song anywhere in the verse and aligning the verse with different parts of the melody, as well as allowing words to be split by line boundaries, and putting stressed syllables in off-beat positions (Turpin 2007, 2015). Yet other obscuring factors might play a role, such as the simultaneous presence of other components, which produce background visual or aural noise. In this way, practices which we have specifically identified as producing 'brilliance' combine with other practices and properties to produce objects which are difficult to perceive and songs which are difficult to decipher. We here argue that perceptual difficulty is a major factor in provoking the experience of Ancestral power.

Transience

Time plays a significant role in the production of brilliance (Morphy 2009). For instance, brilliant Yolngu paintings are viewed only briefly (though the main participants may have a longer experience of them; Morphy 2009). In Central Australian religious practices, sacra such as poles and clap sticks are put away when not used, so that their ritual power is only occasionally manifest (it is also dangerous outside the ritual context), and these sacred objects are made transiently potent again through the application of oil, ochre, and feathers. The pole is briefly touched during the dance, and fluff flies off the men's bodies in motion. Once completed, ceremonial representations in the ground are erased and body designs are scuffed to eliminate the outline (whilst leaving traces to signal that one has been in the ceremonial domain). The materials are potent with Ancestral power that can be only briefly manifested. The temporal constraints are such that white ochre's colour potential for brilliance only becomes so when it is part of ceremonial designs, similarly oil makes the body glisten only until dry, and an object may appear bright only in a particular light. Moreover, the components of the ceremony take on a changing relation as they shift between moments of climax, which can have strong emotional effects and lesser charged interims. Finally, brilliance is contingent on the participants being at a particular moment of knowledge accumulated over many ceremonies, bringing them closer to the Ancestral domain.

At a symbolic level, the transience of the material object contrasts with the permanence of the Altyerre. Songs are brief but their abidance is revealed in the fact that in many verses, verbs are uninflected for tense. Those verses that do have tense marking often have a presentational tense unique to song, or the tense varies between past and non-past with no apparent semantic effect (Koch and Turpin 2008:176).

BRILLIANCE AS SIMULTANEOUSLY EXPERIENCED COMPONENTS

The simultaneity of components

In his analysis of the morphology of feeling in Aboriginal aesthetics, Sutton (1988:59) notes that 'the renewed recombination of a limited number of elements lies behind the austere economy and the consistency of art'. In song performances, the components of melody, rhythmic text, and beating occur at the same time and many also simultaneous with painting or accompanying dance action. In paintings (body, bark, and canvas), individual components that are added successively are sometimes layered on top of one another, adjacent, or overlaid with adornments such as headbands, feathers, and necklaces. Specific verses must be sung to the actions of painting, adornment, and dance. An 'austere economy' and 'consistency' of shapes make this simultaneity different from the everyday simultaneities in ordinary communication, where we communicate with language, aspects of voice, facial expression, and gesture, in how we dress, and so on. Ordinary simultaneities are not often precisely matched in meaning and form, or subject to specific rules of combination like those which are required for the ceremony to be successful.

Visual simultaneity derives from the repetition of similar elements. Designs may have parallel or crossing lines, or circles within circles, often simultaneous to shapes of the dance. Dancers may move in synchrony performing the same actions next to one another, face to face, or in a circle, with each individual's dance a component simultaneous with the other. Tamisari (2000:284) argues that it is in the empathetic connection between dancers, dancing simultaneously that meaning emerges:

As the meaning of the footprints can be said to reside in between, that is in the social, political and emotional bonds they fashion between ancestral events, people and country, I propose that the meaning of Yolngu dancing is between the steps, encountering another person in the intercorporeal space of desire and compassion, love and competition one enters through dancing.

The aesthetic reverberation is further elaborated when the actions of the dancers/painters are echoed in the song text. Thus an important aspect of Aboriginal aesthetic practices is that there are multiple components which are experienced simultaneously. For the knowledgeable participant, this involves identifying what those components are, that is, to 'decipher' form and meaning. Song components include the melody and its coded meaning, the lyrics and their meaning, the rhythm, and the percussive beating. The words of the text must be identified and their meanings established, along with the relations between parallel adjacent lines (cf., examples 1-7). Painted designs must be identified along with their meanings. The knowledgeable participant must also locate the relations of form and meaning cross-modally, that is, between the parts of the verse and the parts of the painting, and between the song as a whole and the painting as a whole. We suggest that the totality of the components and any coded meanings must be experienced in order to confirm proper performance of the ceremony. This will require considerable cognitive processing effort, which in turn creates the psychological conditions in which Ancestral power can become manifest.

Simultaneity and the production of Ancestral power

As argued above, simultaneity of component parts is a factor in brilliance that induces awareness of Ancestral power. In this we follow Catherine Ellis, who says that in ceremony where the naming of Ancestors evokes their presence, 'in order to maintain power at its optimum, overlapping / interlocking / cyclical processes must occur', and 'everything must occur correctly and simultaneously for the full power of the naming to be effective' (Ellis 1984:183). She too argues that many ideas can be communicated simultaneously (Ellis 1985:93). Others have also emphasised simultaneity: Morphy (1998:185) observes that brilliance involves simultaneity of the effects of shine and movement on the object's surface, Young (2011: 64) describes a painting tradition in which 'successions of becomings are represented simultaneously', and Sutton (1988:59) finds another simultaneity in symmetry.

To perform a ceremony is to do what one's Ancestors did, and if carned out well, performance has the potential to influence social cohesion, the outcome of events such as sickness, fights, sporting competitions, and the weather. Performance thus creates a simultaneity of experience between the everyday visible world of the living and the invisible realm of the Ancestors. Deger (2006:115-6) similarly notes how, in moments of brilliance, the everyday and the sublime coalesce, and Young (2006:253) points out the simultaneous animation of body and country. Jennifer Biddle (2007:71-2) writes about Desert acrylic painting:

dots appear to 'float' above surface level, as cloud to ground or figure to shadow. This vacillating simultaneity of surface, depth and back again produces layers, texture, depth. Present and non-present at once, it is the movement between two realms, above and below, that matters.

Ceremonial performance is a contemporary event which is simultaneous with an event performed by Ancestral beings, as well as with an event originally created by the Ancestors in the distant past. A similar temporal blending can be seen with traditional sand stories, which Nancy Munn (1973:88) and Jennifer Green regard as 'a blending of ancestral reality with the "ongoing pattern" or "the microtemporal cycling" of daily life' (2014:51). Both these practices are regarded as having the same ancestral origins (2014: 65). Green (2014:232) observes that characteristics of song are also found in sand stories 'on the more formal end of the spectrum' (that is, explicitly from the Altyerre).

The component parts of a song

At a minimum, the acoustic components of Central Australian (including Arandic) ceremonial song are as follows (Barwick 1989): There is a text or 'verse', usually consisting of two lines. The verse is sung to a specific rhythm. It is sung in rhythmic unison by all singers in a group. The verse is repeated for the duration of a melodic contour, lasting about 30-40 seconds, and the set of repetitions constitutes the 'song item', during which each iteration of the verse may relate in different ways to the melody. There is a short break after this song item and then the same verse is sung again, though it usually relates to the melody in a different way. In Arandic languages, the singing of multiple song items of the one verse is referred to as 'spreading out' the verse (Turpin 2005:95). Ellis et al. (1978:76) call these song items of the same verse a 'small song'. A song item may also involve a particular percussive accompaniment, usually produced with hands or instruments such as clap sticks. We first consider the relation between the rhythm of the sung syllables and the accompaniment.

Most verses have a single percussive accompaniment, but some have two, as illustrated in Fig. 4. Above the text, the percussive beats are shown with 'x', with the vocal rhythm on top (each note matching a syllable). In the 'slow duple' accompaniment of this song every percussive beat coincides with a syllable. In the 'fast triple' accompaniment some percussive beats coincide with syllables (beats 1, 4, 7, and 10) and others do not. Thus, a single song text has a consistent vocal rhythm but can have two different metres in song items of the one small song. In Fig. 4, the one verse, represented here with just one of its lines 'The coveted smooth designs were put on', has two different accompaniments: a slow duple for some song items and fast triple for others.

Juxtaposing a sequence of different metres for the one verse (with one rhythmic vocal line) can have an interesting acoustic effect, like an optical illusion where the song is perceived in brief moments as both triple and duple.

In some song sets, the percussive beating ceases and then returns, with the cessation signalled in some Warlpiri song sets by a brief tremolo of the clap sticks. The percussive beat can change tempo to double speed, with clapping also on the off-beat, then return to single-beat clapping. These changes in the percussive line change the relations between the vocal line, percussive line and dancing, and can signal changes in the dancing.

[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]

The vocal line and the beats form sequences which usually begin and end at the same point. However, in rare cases, a pair of lines matches a sequence of 15 beats, and so each line coincides with 7.5 beats. For example, Figure 5 shows a verse whose lines A and B are both 7.5 beats and so the repetition of each line differs, producing a structure of A1 A2 B1 B2 in terms of the alignment of vocal line with beat.

The repeating text is set to the sung rhythms in regulated ways, aligning the left edges of words to the left edges of rhythmic units (Turpin and Laughren 2013). This can result in altering the stress patterns of words, and setting the last (unstressed) syllable of a word to a long note. Since there is also vowel modification, this can make it difficult to discern words. For example, in Fig. 6, the first word ngurrungka is sung as ngirringka:, with stress and lengthening on the final syllable and articulation of the back vowel 'u' to a front vowel 'i'.

The vocal line and the percussive components have a shifting relation to the melody (Barwick 1989, 1990:61; Ellis 1985; Treloyn 2007; Tunstill 1995). In Fig. 6, the rhythmic text repeats in an AABB pattern while being performed to a longer melody. It can be seen that the pitch of each syllable often differs when it is repeated. For example, the first syllable of line A, ngi, is sung to A flat in its first iteration, F in its second and thereafter. The first syllable of line B, pa, is sung to A flat in its first and F thereafter. The percussive accompaniment is at every quarter beat for the first two lines (AA) and then goes to half speed until the end of the song (the quarter note clap in bar two is not significant).

Our aim in this section has been to show that while each auditory component of a song is in itself relatively simple, the varying combinations of components with one another are complex. The singer must devote considerable effort to maintaining the structure of each component whilst interlocking them in different ways, and without 'getting lost' (the term used by Ellis 1985, 1997). The fieldworker herself might co-experience cognitive satisfaction discerning words and their significance in the moment of ceremony, experienced aha-moments, something similarly described by field linguist Ken Hale (1984:259):

[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]

[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]

I can report from my own personal experience that arriving at even the most elementary understanding of a traditional centralian couplet generates a feeling of considerable intellectual joy. Indeed, the mere discovery, unaided, that the yurrampi verses which I heard at Yuendumu were not in Warlpiri but in an Arandic language (a process which took me two days, I must admit with some chagrin) was nothing short of thrilling.

Ellis on 'iridescence' in Australian song

Ellis (1984, 1985) notes the presence of multiple simultaneous components such as verse, melody and percussion, and proposes that the listener attends to a single component and then 'flips' to another: 'at first one, then another [pattern] occupies the centre of attention' (Ellis 1984:160). This is a type of musical perception in which one component is heard as figure to the other components as ground, and she says that the listener can experience first one and then another component as the figure, 'flipping' between figures (as described in relation to multiple metres above). She says this is like visual iridescence, where different colours appear on an object depending on angle of perception.

Although Ellis is right that listeners can separate out one component for attention as figure to ground, there is evidence that listeners can also hear different components at the same time. Thus, Leonard Meyer (1956:186) argues that polyphonic music can have multiple figures without a ground, and Pressing, Summers and Magill (1996:1127) say that a musical polyrhythm can be perceptually organized as a combined pattern instead of a figure-ground relation. Whether a figure-ground relationship emerges in listening also depends on compositional practice (Deutsch 2007:4473), individual listener variation (Handel 1984:481), and performance (Tsur 2012). Furthermore, an apparent figure-ground relation might involve only conscious perception, but at the same time, listeners might be unconsciously processing the other simultaneous background components (Huron 2006:42). All this suggests that while Ellis might be right that listeners hear figure to ground and can flip them, they might also be listening in a more complex way involving greater simultaneity of components. In either case, the important point for our purposes is that listening to Aboriginal song involves complex cognition, and focused attention. In experimental tests, attention to a created temporal interval has been found to be associated with the slowing of subjective time (Coull et al. 2011; Jakubowski et al. 2015; Tse et al. 2004). Perhaps attention to musical form, given the relation between musical and temporal cognition, has this effect of also slowing subjective time. Furthermore, studies of attention suggest that perception is facilitated for emotionally significant stimuli (Vuilleumier 2009), such as the stimuli of ritual, while emotional responses to music can also be accentuated by attention to the music (Grewe et al. 2007). Commenting on the transformational effect of music, Ellis writes that the perception of Aboriginal music is 'significant in changing the participant's awareness of the musical event, of his own personality, and of the nature of the time experience through which he is passing' (Ellis 1984:168).

Our interpretation of Ellis fits with Linda Barwick's development of Ellis's ideas, which retains the emphasis on selective perception of parts, while emphasizing a grasp of the whole:

The simultaneous presentation of independently cycling temporal structures has an iridescent quality, where first one structural aspect, then another, may be in the foreground of our consciousness. Grasping the whole requires abandoning analytical listening and may be a profoundly disorienting experience. How much more overwhelming when to the independent auditory patternings of melody, rhythm, beating, and text are added, in ceremony, the simultaneous presentation of information through dance and design, sometimes partially obscured by flickering firelight and smoke! (Barwick 2000:332)

Noting that the song components take on a combined meaning greater than the aggregate meaning of the parts (Barwick 2005:2), Barwick describes an 'inductive space' between the components which 'leaves room for ambiguity and slippage'. Sally Treloyn (2007:91, 94) adds that this 'invites listeners to consider the connection or equivalence between distinct elements' ... 'in which relationships between dancers, singers, ancestral beings and listeners are formed'.

We suggest that the demands of listening to this music involve considerable cognitive effort, especially since the various components are perceived simultaneously. Note that the singers must do this in order to coordinate the parts (as noted above) but that it is possible that listeners do the same, and indeed necessary for expert listeners in order to be sure the songs have been performed correctly. We thus expand the notion of 'iridescent' to describe the complex combination of parts similar to Susanne Langer's description of music as 'iridescent. Its values crowd each other, its symbols are inexhaustible' (1948:194).

Components can be simultaneous in memory

In working memory, a person integrates their current perceptions with their long-term memory; working memory contains what we are attending to at any time, whether consciously or unconsciously, and it has a very limited capacity. This means that components of ceremony can be processed simultaneously, even if they are not performed at the same time (but are close in time, as are adjacent lines of a verse). Australian Aboriginal songs are short enough that all the words of a verse, usually consisting of two separate lines, could all be held at the same time in working memory.(5) This is of particular relevance when considering the characteristic relation between adjacent lines. A verse is usually two lines A and B, with line A repeated (with minor variations) before line B, also repeated, in an A1-A2-B1-B2 pattern. We suggest that line A1 can be held in working memory while line A2 is sung and brought into working memory alongside A1, then A2 is held in working memory while line B1 is sung, and then B1 held in memory while line B2 is sung. This is the case even for the longest of verses, such as those in (8). If the adjacent lines can both be held at the same time in working memory, the parallelism between them can be established in working memory (as Fabb 2015 argues is generally true of parallelism in poetry). Thus, though the lines are sequential in performance, they are simultaneously held in working memory.
(8.)
Al    Ngwenty-ngwentyarl arrernerlhenhek
A2    Ngwenty-ngwentyarl arrernerlhenhek
Bl    Anwekakerrenharl arlkeny malangkarl
      alimarrarnkek
B2    Anwekakerrenharl arlkeny malangkarl
      alimarrarnkek

(8.)
Al    Adorning ourselves with white ochre
A2    Adorning ourselves with white ochre
Bl    Our lovely designs glistened

B2    Our lovely designs glistened

      (Turpin 2015:79, Alyawarr)


Long-term memory enables another kind of simultaneity between components which are separated in time. Thus, Ellis (1984:159) notes that components may be said by the participants to be 'at the same time' when they are not at the same clock-time, but are conceived of as simultaneous and brought together in memory. As Morphy (2009:17) argues, long-term memory (the culture's 'virtual archive') plays a major role in creating the experience of brilliance. Drawing on Daniele Klapproth's analysis of Central Australian narrative structure, Jennifer Green (2014:43-4) highlights 'the blurring of the distinction between fiction and non-fiction and the potential for humans to coexist simultaneously in their human and totemic forms (Klapproth 2004:76)' as 'one of the keys to understanding Altyerr as an overarching creative principle'.

Memory also plays a role in producing tactile simultaneities through intermodal and interpersonal transpositions, processes derived from the experience of having paint put on one's own body, and of putting it on another's body. Seeing someone painted can evoke the feeling of being painted, and of painting somebody else's body or other pictorical surface like a canvas of bark. Vice versa, images of the design can evoke the sensation of touch. Other intersubjective transpositions involve a 'bodily exchange between human and Ancestor', which, as Biddle (2007:75) explains, may be understood as an 'epiphany of touch'. A structural binarity or dual unity that inherently presents simultaneity is the division into moieties. Ceremonial roles and ritual objects are often divided along the moiety division. Even when only one moiety is involved in ritual action or represented, it implies the absence of the other. This is apparent in Warlpiri women's performance (Laughren et al. 2011).

Arguably then, both working and long-term memory are not only intertwined in a general sense with creativity, they are also more specifically pivotal to the production of complex simultaneities.

Songs which are simultaneous with body painting and dancing

In women's ceremonies, particular songs are performed when painting the body in order 'to activate the power of the Dreaming' (see also Morais 1998:465 on Warlpiri). In both Arandic and Warlpiri women's ceremonies, there must be no change of verse before the completion of the relevant part of the visual design or the dance, that is, the visual component must coincide with the 'small song' (Turpin 2005:95). With complex dance actions, the small song may last up to 10 minutes. In Warlpiri ceremony, the 'small song', which is made up of repeated song items of a verse, increases in intensity (of dance actions, dynamics, and tempo) as the performance approaches the climax of the Ancestral action, such as closing in on the ceremonial pole or snatching the ceremonial board (Laughren et al. 2011).(6) Morphy (2016, personal communication) likewise found that increased intensity produces brilliance in Yolngu ceremony. Tamisari (2014) argues that participation in dance, where crucially people dance together empathetically, offers access to the 'numinous'.

Correspondence of the semantics of verse and associated dance action constitutes another form of simultaneity. For example, there are verses that must be sung during the application of fat or oil, as in (9). Note that here 'oil' is referred to both with its everyday word anter and with its ceremonial word rtway.
(9.)
Anterel arrernerl-anem  Preparing our bodies with oil
Rtwayel arrernerl-anem  Preparing our bodies with healing oil
                        (Turpin and Ross 2013:27, Alyawarr)


Verses about white ochre must be sung when white ochre is applied, and verses about yellow ochre when yellow is applied (Gallagher et al. 2014). As one Alyawarr singer put it, 'the designs tell you what song to sing' (Katie Kemarr 2007 personal communication).

Verses that have an accompanying action frequently have a text that refers to this action or the objects involved in this action (Barwick 2006). For example, the verse in (10) has an accompanying action where the dancers look to the east.
(10.)
Akngirrerlarle arerl-an      Looking to the east
A herrk-aherrkele arerl-ane  In the early morning
                             (Turpin 2005, app. 1:71)


In some Arandic languages, a woman complete with ritual designs and adornment can be referred to as wetyerrpetyerrpe. Such a person is ready to dance and inspires praise and awe. In everyday speech, the word means 'full' (Tuipin and Green 2011:305). This word occurs in a number of verses, such as that in (11). It is sung when the painting is complete.
(11.)
Welye-le wetyerrpetyerrpe     In the shade all painted up
Welye-le wetyerrpe-wetyerrpe  In the shade all painted up
                              (Turpin 2005, app. 1:176)


We have not encountered any verses about singing or playing clapsticks in Warlpiri or Arandic ceremonial songs (although there are references to divas or bards). It thus appears that verses that refer to an aspect of ceremonial performance only refer to the dance and visual domain, and not the auditory domain. In other words, simultaneities are preferentially distributed in different modalities. The simultaneities across modalities are so essential as to merit further illustration.

Ancestral designs are not only painted on the performers' skin, but also on the 'symbolically multi-referential paraphernalia, which the dancers wield or emphasize' (Morais 1998:465). In addition to understanding the meanings of these ritual objects in context, the dance involves other distinct components presented simultaneously. These components include the structure and symbolism of 'mimetic' dance actions (Dail-Jones 1998), and the relationship of movement to stationary objects, such as the ritual pole, cardinal directions, and any other symbolic spaces in the dance arena. In the Warlpiri region, a fundamental feature is the division of dancers in terms of the binary kinship categories of managers and owners. For the participant or observer, whether one is manager or owner varies depending on where they themselves fit into this system.

Movements of the body coincide with percussive beats. Dancers move to double speed when the beating does and likewise reduce speed as soon as the beating does. The dances are not memorized but recreated from 'singing, myths and manager's directions' (Morais 1998:467). Dance is an interlocking of multiple components in multiple possible ways realized in the moment of performance.

Semantic parallelism, or correspondence of meaning, is also found in the song set as a whole, involving larger-scale simultaneities. This is most evident in the travelling songs, where the song set is coterminous with the travels of particular Ancestral characters. In many central Australian languages, the word for melodic contour refers to the essence of an Ancestral being, as well as to the concepts 'taste', 'scent,' and 'skin name' (subsection) (Barwick and Marett 1995; Ellis et al. 1978; Turpin, 2005). Similarly, a ceremonial body design is worn throughout the entire song set, and is never removed before the ceremony finishes though it may be covered by a shirt. (Some song sets are associated with more than one design, as these usually relate to different regions within a single territory.)

The complex surface

Brilliant practices sometimes involve the addition of layers (paint, fluff, headbands, etc.) to a surface such as the skin, an artefact, or the ground. The surface of the ground can also be affected by digging into it, sticking a pole into it, wiping it clean, or stamping on the ground and raising dust. Green (2014:66) observes that 'Arandic people regard the ground as ... simultaneously a drawing surface and a conduit which connects the visible surface world to that of the ancestors' who 'are covered over, but [they] can be recalled at any time.' (A similar claim is made for Balgo art by Christine Watson 2003.)

These ways of making a surface complex are ways of encouraging Ancestors to emerge through that surface. As Franca Tamisari explained (2002), Ancestral beings originally affected the surface by imprinting the ground by hitting, piercing, or standing an object up in it, which in combination with naming created the topographical features. We have already seen how standing a ceremonial pole into the ground is a feature of the dance and lyrics in central Australian song (2), (5). Piercing is also a feature of the dances and verses of ceremonial songs, as in (12).
(12.)
Namaywengkel rnternepernem    The spirit women are dancing
Taty-tatyelarl rnternepernem  With dancing sticks they are piercing
                              the air
                              (Campbell et al. 2015:40, Anmatyerr)


Alyawarr ceremonial designs represent different estates (Moyle 1983), and typically one wears the designs of one's father's father's estate in ceremony. Mappings of kin on the body can be seen in other contexts too, including sign language, and twitches in particular parts of the body, which are said to signal the approach of particular kin (Turpin et al. 2013:13). Kin, country, and totems are considered parts of a person (Wilkins 1993:74), and it is these parts of the self that, captured by the brilliance of the design, transitorily emerge from within the body through the skin. In other words, ceremonial body designs symbolically change the surface of the body so that one's father's father's country 'emerges' from the self and becomes visible. As Tamisari (2000:278) says, 'knowledge associated with country is thus absorbed and transmitted through the body'. Body paintings are typically produced with a layer of oil on top of which colours are applied in repetitive patterns, without, however, superimposition. Others have noted the 'depth of field' that this technique produces (Morphy 1998:187; Deger 2006:194). There is an implied relation between such perceptual depth and the spiritual and epistemic depth of the Ancestral order, along with Ancestral movements between the invisible realm below the surface of the land, objects and body, and the outer visible realm.

Human action is integral to this dynamic, as ethnographers have long shown. Deger (2006:193) says that 'Yolngu undertake the cultural work of painting or singing to actively open up and reconfigure spaces between the subject, image and imaged', and referring to Biddle (2001), she emphasizes the 'bidirectional, intersubjective and intercorporeal dynamics that underpin Warlpiri cultural practice'. Similarly John Morton (1989:285) describes the pressing of objects onto the initiate's body so that power penetrates deep inside his 'guts', along with other movements across a surface including where the Ancestor takes some object out of his body. There are parallels in other Indigenous lifeworlds. This includes 'the self-decoration of Papuan Hageners' described by Young (2011:365), 'whose social efficacy is brought out from within and displayed on their skins by oiling and adding paints, feathers, and leaves'. Another example is the Abelam tradition of attaching feathers, leaves, and shells to the body, which binds 'the body to the external environment', producing continuity with the Ancestors (Hauser-Schaublin 1996:103).

BRILLIANCE AS AN INSTANCE OF THE SUBLIME

In this paper, we have offered extensive evidence for two aesthetic characteristics of Aboriginal ceremony and its component parts. One is a perceptual brilliance, involving visual brightness (including whiteness, iridescence, etc.). The other is the conceptual brilliance of complex simultaneities in ceremony, where body, song, and dance make up a plethora of component parts that are variously combined, often expressing similar meanings in different ways, but also producing different meanings that must be grasped at the same time. Here, the cognitive and emotional responsiveness of performers and skilled listeners is intensified. Morphy used the term 'brilliance' to describe the characteristic of visual brightness. We suggest that 'brilliance' might be extended to include the componential complexity of ceremonial and artistic practices and argued that this is implicit in much previous discussion, including through the use of brightness-related terms such as 'iridescent' to describe componential complexity. Furthermore, we see that both visual brightness and componential complexity produce psychological effects on participants (and hence are likely to work in similar ways) that create a sense of Ancestral power as tangibly present. Such epiphany in Aboriginal aesthetic experience can be usefully thought of as an instantiation of the sublime. Both the sublime and Aboriginal brilliance are culturally specific experiences, which may be based on universals of human psychology. In relating the experience of brilliance to the experience of the sublime, we follow Douglas Dalton's (1996) use of the Kantian sublime in his account of brilliant Rawa shell and tooth ornaments

According to Dacher Keltner and Jonathan Haidt (2003), awe is the central emotion in experiences of the sublime; just as has been argued for the sublime, awe can be triggered by various kinds of extremity. The two characteristics of Aboriginal ceremony we have highlighted can be considered as kinds of extremity. The first, perceptual brightness and associated phenomena, is pivotal to the experience of Ancestral power in a way similar to how in other cultures brightness is associated with awe, the sublime and so on. The layered complexity of ceremony produces another kind of extremity--the amount that must be mentally processed by the participant in order to fit all the parts together. Arguably, the attention and effort required to do so produce psychological effects identified with the sublime, awe, or Ancestral power. Edmund Burke (1987:78) perceived exactly this in the 'descriptions in the poets and orators, which owe their sublimity to a richness and profusion of images' that dazzle the mind and may overwhelm it.

In a related approach to the sublime, David Miall and Don Kuiken (1994) draw on Viktor Shklovsky's idea of defamiliarization: 'The technique of art is to make objects "unfamiliar," to make forms difficult, to increase the difficulty and length of perception', and thereby 'to make one feel things, to make the stone stony' (Shklovsky 1965:12, cited in Miall and Kuiken 1994:391). They discuss 'foregrounding' as a way of deploying the complexity of literary form to effect a 'defamiliarization' in the reader, and to thereby invoke the sublime. We suggest that the componential complexity of Aboriginal ceremony constitutes a kind of foregrounding, which produces defamiliarization in the participant in which the objects, events, and utterances are no longer ordinary, but take on Ancestral power. Miall (2007:155) writes that 'the sublime represents an extreme mode of defamiliarization', where the complexity of cognitions demanded from the reader push that reader into a powerful emotional and epistemic experience. Some emotions such as sadness may enhance the 'sublime' effects of a ceremony and sadness may arise when the performance recalls past performances with people now dead; Sikora, Kuiken and Miall (2010) and Kuiken and Oliver (2013) argue that sad emotions increase the attention to the text and hence foregrounding and defamiliarization. The result is an intense emotional and epistemic experience.

Defamiliarization has been observed in the alternate register used during initiation. Morton describes Aranda 'shame talk' or Warlpiri 'upside-down language', where the initiate is first confused and humiliated by the use of the language until he experiences poetic joy 'in a sudden flash of insight' (Morton 1989:291; Hale 1971:475, 482). We suggest that the intensity that is said to be experienced by the participant is related to the sublime, as part of a broader family of experiences which involve powerfully felt moments of insight--as William James (1902) noted, these include mystical experiences. Thus, there may be a distinction between a general feeling of Ancestral power which may hold throughout a ceremony, and distinct special moments. These special moments come as a surprise. Where the insight produces a fluency in processing, as the task of understanding becomes suddenly easier, then we would expect various 'fluency effects' such as a sense of pleasure, of the truth of what is being represented, and so on (as described by Reber et al. 2004), and these may all contribute to the emotional and epistemic effects of brilliance. Furthermore, we note that moments of insight might be more likely when the subject is put to greater processing effort, as we have argued is the case in ceremony. In turn, greater cognitive effort raises the perception of surprise, as Mark Foster and Meadhbh Keane (2015:80) suggest.

In conclusion, we suggest that the two kinds of brilliance--perceptual and conceptual--both involve the experiencing subject in psychological processes that have elsewhere been associated with awe, the sublime, and moments of surprise and insight. These culturally specific practices thus draw on general aspects of human psychology.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the many Arandic and Warlpiri people who have shared their traditional songs with Myfany Turpin. In particular, M.K. Turner, Agnes Abbott of Alice Springs, Katie and Mary Kemarr from Utopia, Clarrie Kemarr from Ti-Tree, Daisy Kemarre, Amy Ngamperle, and Tommy Thompson from Barrow Creek, and Lucy, Leah, Peggy and Marilyn Nampijinpa, Dora, Lady and Maisy Napaljarri and Kathy Nangala from Willowra. We thank Howard Morphy, Peter Sutton and Jennifer Green for providing very detailed comments and criticisms and for pointing us to other relevant work. We also thank Linda Barwick, David Nash, Jane Simpson, an anonymous reviewer and the editors for their feeback. We thank Yun Yi Calista Yeoh for the musical transcription in Fig. 6. Fabb's research for this article was funded by the Leverhulme Trust as part of a Major Research Fellowship titled 'Epiphanies in literature: a psychological and literary linguistic account'. Turpin's research for this article was funded by the Australian Research Council (FT140100783, DP 1092887) and the Hans Rausing Endangered Languages Documentation Programme (IPF0100, IPF0173).

NOTES

(1.) Arandic is a language dialect chain which includes Arrernte. Alyawarr, Anmatyerr, and Kaytetyc. For words in these languages, and Warlpiri, we follow the standard orthography used for each of these languages. For pan-Arandic words, we use the Kaytetye orthography (Turpin and Ross 2013).

(2.) Note that in Anbarra, -gungaltja 'light' can include red if it is 'highly saturated' and 'brilliant' (Jones and Meehan 1978:27). Morphy (2016, personal communication) also notes the use of glistening and naturally burnishing red in Yolngu ceremony.

(3.) Archive number of the field recording held at the Australian Institute for Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies archive, Canberra, Australia.

(4.) Both words may be compounds based on a putative form iylerlk 'white, bright': iylerlk + arr plus the relativiser -arr 'headband'; and iylerlk plus annge 'eye' 'Sandover lilly'.

(5.) See Fabb (2015) for discussion of poetic lines and their fit to working memory capacity. Baddeley (2012:16) estimates that about 15 words of English prose can fit as a whole. There is no measure for Australian Aboriginal songs (or languages) but given the greater complexity of the words, we would expect fewer words to be held at any one time; on the other hand, lines of verses do have relatively few words. Note that Miller's 'magic number seven' is no longer considered valid as a measure of working memory capacity by psychologists of memory (Cowan 2000, Miller 1956).

(6.) Morphy similarly observes a gradual build up in Yolngu ceremonial performance to a moment of catharsis (2016. personal communication).

REFERENCES

AKERMAN, K. and J. STANTON. 1994. Riji and Jakuli: Kimberley Pearl Shell in Aboriginal Australia. Darwin. Australia: Northern Territory Museum of Arts and Sciences.

ANDERSON, C. 2013. Renaissance Architecture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

BADDELEY, A. 2012. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 1-29.

BARWICK, L. 1989. Creative (ir)regularities: The intermeshing of text and melody in performance of Central Australian Song. Australian Aboriginal Studies 1: 12-28.

--. 1990. Central Australian women's ritual music: Knowing through analysis versus knowing through performance. Yearbook for Traditional Music 22: 60-79.

--. 2000. Song as an Indigenous Art. In S. Kleinert and M. Neale (eds), The Oxford Companion to Aboriginal Art and Culture. Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press, pp. 328-335.

--. 2005. Performance, aesthetics, experience: Thoughts on Yawulyu Mungamunga songs. In E. Mackinlay, D. Collins and S. Owens (eds), Aesthetics and Experience in Music Performance. Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars Press, pp. 1-18.

--. 2006. The Virtuous Circle of Tradition and Practice. Unpublished paper presented at the 29th National Conference of the Musicological Society of Australia. Armidale, Australia.

BARWICK, L., and A. MARETT. 1995. Introduction. In L. Barwick, A. Marett and G. Tunstill (eds.) The Essence of Singing and the Substance of Song. Recent Responses to the Aboriginal Performing Arts and Other Essays in Honour of Catherine Ellis. pp. 1-10. Sydney. Australia: University of Sydney (Oceania Monograph 46).

BECKER, J. 1994. Music and trance. Leonardo Music Journal 4: 41-51.

BIDDLE, J. L. 2001. Inscribing identity: Skin as country in the Central Desert. In A. Lee and C. Poynton (eds), Thinking through the Skin. London. UK: Routledge. pp. 177-193.

--. 2007. Breasts, Bodies, Canvas: Central Desert Art as Experience. Sydney, Australia: University of South Wales Press.

BURKE, E. 1987. In J. T. Boulton (ed), A Philosophical Enquiry into the Origin of our Ideas of the Sublime and Beautiful, Revised ed. Oxford. UK: Blackwell.

CAMPBELL, A., C. KEMARR. J. GREEN and M. CAREW. 2015. Mer Angenty-warn alhem. Travelling to Angenty Country. Darwin, Australia: N.T. Batchelor Press.

COULL, J. T., R.-K. CHENG and W. H. MECK. 2011. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 3-25.

COWAN, N. 2000. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 87-185.

DAIL-JONES, M. 1998. Warlpiri dance. In S. J. Cohen (ed), The International Encyclopedia of Dance. Vol. 1. New York. NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 227-229.

DALTON, D. M. 1996. The aesthetic of the sublime: An interpretation of Rawa Shell valuable symbolism. American Ethnologist 23(2): 393-415.

DEGER, J. 2006. Shimmering Screens: Making Media in an Aboriginal Community. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

DEUTSCH, D. 2007. Music perception. In Listening in the World: Behavioral and Neurobiological Bases of Complex-Sound Perception. Frontiers of Bioscience (special issue). Vol. 12. pp. 4473-4482.

DODWELL, C. R. 1993. The Pictorial Arts of the West 800-1200. New Haven. CT: Yale University Press.

DUSSART, F. 1988. Women's acrylic paintings from Yuendumu. In M. West (ed), The Inspired Dream: Life as Art in Aboriginal Australia. Brisbane, Australia: Queensland Art Gallery, pp. 35-40.

ELLIS, C. J. 1984. Time consciousness of aboriginal performers. In J. C. Kassler and J. Stubington (eds), Problems and Solutions. Occasional Essays in Musicology Presented to Alice M. Moyle. Sydney, Australia: Hale and Iremonger, pp. 149-185.

--. 1985. Aboriginal Music. Education for Living. St. Lucia Queensland. Australia: University of Queensland Press.

--. 1997. Understanding the profound structural knowledge of Central Australian performers from the perspective of T.G.H. Strehlow. Strehlow Research Centre Occasional Papers 1: 57-78.

ELLIS, C. J., A. A. M. ELLIS, M. TUR and A. MCCARDELL. 1978. Classification of sounds in Pitjantjatjaraspeaking area. In L. A. Hiatt (ed), Australian Aboriginal Concepts. Canberra, Australia; Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies Press, pp. 68-80.

FABB, N. 2015. What is Poetry? Language and Memory in the Poems of the World. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

FOSTER, M. I. and M. T. KEANE. 2015. Why some surprises are more surprising than others: Surprise as a metacognitive sense of explanatory difficulty. Cognitive Psychology 81: 74-116.

GALLAGHER, C., P. BROWN. G. CURRAN and B. MARTIN. 2014. .Jardiwanpa yawulyu. Warlpiri women's songs from Yuendumu. Darwin, Australia: Batchelor Press.

GREEN, J. 2014. Drawn from the Ground: Sound, Sign and Inscription in Central Australian Sand Stories. Cambridge. UK: Cambridge University Press.

GREWE, O., F. NAGEL. R. KOPIEZ and E. ALTENMULLER. 2007. Listening to music as a re-creative process: Physiological, psychological, and psychoacoustical correlates of chills and strong emotions. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 24(3): 297-314.

HALE, K. 1971. A note on a Walbiri tradition of antonymy. In D. D. Steinberg and L. A. Jakobovits (eds), Semantics. Cambridge. UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 472-82.

--. 1984. Remarks on creativity in aboriginal verse. In J. C. Kassler and J. Stubington (eds), Problems and Solutions. Occasional Essays in Musicology Presented to Alice M. Moyle. Sydney. Australia: Hale and Iremonger, pp. 254-262.

HANDEL, S. 1984. Using polyrhythms to study rhythm. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 1(4): 465-484.

HAUSER-SCHAUBLIN, B. 1996. The thrill of the line, the string, and the frond, or why the Abelam are a non-cloth culture. Oceania 67(2): 81-106.

HUNZINGER, C. 2015. Wonder. In P. Destree and P. Murray (eds), A Companion to Ancient Aesthetics. Maiden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 422-437.

HURON, D. 2006. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

INGRAM, C. 2013. Understanding musical participation: Big song singers in Kam villages. In I. Russell and C. Ingram (eds), Taking Part in Music. Aberdeen. Scotland: Aberdeen University Press, pp. 53-65.

JAEGER, C. S. 2010. Introduction. In C. S. JAEGER (ed.) Magnificence and the Sublime in Medieval Aesthetics. Art, Architecture, Literature, Music. 1-16. London. UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

JAKUBOWSKI, K., A. HALPERN, M. GRIERSON and L. STEWART. 2015. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on chosen tempi for familiar melodies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22(2): 559-565.

JAMES, W. 1902. The Varieties of Religious Experience. A Study in Human Nature. New York, NY: Longmans. Green and Co.

JONES, R. and B. MEEHAN. 1978. Anbarra concept of colour. In L. R. Hiatt (ed), Australian Aboriginal Concepts. Canberra, Australia: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies Press, pp. 20-39.

JORDANIA, J. 2011. Why Do People Sing? Music in Human Evolution. Melbourne, Australia: Logos.

KELTNER, D. and J. HAIDT. 2003. Approaching awe. a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion 17(2): 297-314.

KJELLGREN, E. 2007. Oceania: Art of the Pacific Islands in the Metropolitan Museum of Art. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

KLAPPROTH, D. M. 2004. Narrative as Social Practice: Anglo-Western and Australian Aboriginal Oral Traditions. Berlin, Germany, New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.

KOCH, G. and M. TURPIN. 2008. The language of aboriginal songs. In C. Bowern. B. Evans and L. Miceli (eds), Morphology and Language History. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 167-183.

KUIKEN, D. and M. B. OLIVER. 2013. Aesthetic engagement during moments of suffering. Scientific Study of Literature 3(2): 294-321.

LANGER, S. K. 1948. Philosophy in a New Key. A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite and Art. New York, NY: New American Library.

LAUGHREN, M., M. TURPIN. and H. MORTON. 2011. Yawulyu Wirliyajarrayi-wardingkiki: ngatijirri, ngapa. Willowra songlines. 60 min Ethnographic Film of Warlpiri Women's Ceremony. Includes Extras of Additional Singing and Visits to Country. [AIATSIS Accession 782.42099429 WIL DVD].

LEEDING, V. J. 1989. Anindiyakwa Phonology and Morphology. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Sydney. Sydney, Australia.

MEYER, L. B. 1956. Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

MIALL, D. S. 2007. Foregrounding and the sublime: Shelley in Chamonix. Language and Literature 16(2): 155-168.

MIALL, D. S. and D. KUIKEN. 1994. Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and affect: Response to literary stories. Poetics 22: 389-407.

MILLER, G. A. 1956. The magical number seven, plus or minus two. Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 101: 343-352.

MILLER, P. C. 2000. 'The little blue flower is red': Relics and the poetizing of the body. Journal of Early Christian Studies 8(2): 213-236.

MORAIS, M. J. 1998. Warlpiri dance, Central Australia. In J. W. Love and A. L. Kaeppler (eds), Australia and the Pacific Islands, The Garland Encyclopedia of World Music. Vol. 9. New York, NY: Garland Pub, pp. 464-67.

MORPHY, H. 1989. From dull to brilliant: The aesthetics of spiritual power among the Yolngu. Man New Series 24(1): 21-0.

--. 1998. Aboriginal Art. London, UK: Phaidon.

--. 2005. Style and meaning: Abelam art through Yolngu eyes: The 1998 Anthony forge lecture. RES: Anthropology and Aesthetics 47: 209-230.

--. 2009. Art as a mode of action. Some problems with Gell's art and agency. Journal of Material Culture 14(1): 5-27.

MORPHY, H. and J. CARTY. 2015. Understanding country. In G. Sculthorpe. J. Carty, H. Morphy, M. Nugent, I. Coates, L. Bolton and J. Jones (eds), Indigenous Australia: Enduring Civilisation. London, UK: British Museum Press, pp. 20-1 17.

MORTON, J. 1989. Singing subjects and sacred objects: A psychological interpretation of the 'transformation of subjects into objects' in Central Australian Myth. Oceania 59(4): 280-298.

MOYLE, R. 1983. Songs, ceremonies and sites: the Agharringa case. In M. Langton and N. Peterson (eds), Aborigines, Land and Land Rights. Canberra, Australia: Australian Institute for Aboriginal Studies, pp. 66-93.

MUNN, N. D. 1973. Walbiri Iconography. Graphic Representation and Cultural Symbolism in a Central Australian Society. Ithaca. NY; London, UK: Cornell University Press.

NEFF, D. L. 1987. Aesthetics and power in Pambin Tullal: A possession ritual of rural Kerala. Ethnology 26(1): 63-71.

NISBETT, R. E. and T. D. WILSON. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84(3): 231-259.

PRESSING, J., J. SUMMERS and J. MAGILL. 1996. Cognitive multiplicity in polyrhythmic pattern performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22(5): 1127-1148.

REBER. R., N. SCHWARZ and P. WINKIELMAN. 2004. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review 8(4): 364-382.

ROSCOE, P. B. 1995. Of power and menace: Sepik art as an affecting presence. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 1(1): 1-22.

SHKLOVSKY, V. 1965. Art as technique. In L. T. Lemon and M. J. Reis (eds) (eds. and trans.), Russian Formalist Criticism: Four Essays. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 3-57.

SIKORA, S., D. KUIKEN and D. MIALL. 2010. An uncommon resonance: The influence of loss on expressive reading. Empricial Studies of the Arts 28(2): 135-153.

STANNER, W. E. H. 1956. The dreaming. In T. A. G. Hungerford (ed), Australian Signpost: An Anthology. Melbourne. Australia: F. W. Cheshire, pp. 51-65.

SUTTON, P. 1988. The morphology of feeling. In P. Sutton (ed), Dreamings: The Art of Aboriginal Australia. New York, NY; Melbourne. Australia: The Asia Society Galleries & George Braziller; Viking/Penguin, pp. 59-88.

SUTTON, P. and M. SNOW. 2015. Iridescence. The Play of Colours. Port Melbourne, Australia: Thames and Hudson.

TAMISARI, F. 2000. The meaning of the steps is in between: Dancing and the curse of compliments. The Australian Journal of Anthropology 11(3): 274-286.

--. 2002. Names and naming: Speaking forms into place. In L. Hercus, F. Hodges and J. Simpson (eds), The Land Is a Map: Placenames of Indigenous Origin in Australia. Canberra, Australia: Pandanus Books, pp. 87-102.

--. 2014. Feeling, motion and attention in the display of emotions in Yolngu law, song, and dance performance. Journal for the Anthropological Study of Human Movement 21(2): 1-22.

TRELOYN, S. 2007. Flesh with country: Juxtaposition and minimal contrast in the construction and melodic treatment of Jadmi song texts. Australian Aboriginal Studies 2: 90-199.

TSE, P. U., J. INTRILIGATOR. J. RIVEST and P. CAVANAGH. 2004. Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Perception & Psychophysics 66(7): 1171-1189.

TSUR, R. 2012. Playing by Ear and the Tip of the Tongue: Precategorial Information in Poetry. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.

TUNSTILL, G. 1995. Learning Pitjantjatjara songs. In L. Barwick, A. Marett and G. Tunstill (eds), The Essence of Singing and the Substance of Song. Recent Responses to the Aboriginal Performing Arts and Other Essays in Honour of Catherine Ellis. Sydney, Australia: University of Sydney, pp. 59-74.

TURP1N, M. 2005. Form and Meaning of Akwelye: A Kaytetye Women's Song Series from Central Australia. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Sydney.

--. 2007. Artfully hidden: Text and rhythm in a Central Australian Aboriginal song series. Musicology Australia 29: 93-108.

--. 2015. Alyawarr Women's song-poetry of Central Australia. Australian Aboriginal Studies 2015(1): 66-96.

TURPIN, M. and J. GREEN. 2011. Trading in terms: Alternate registers and linguistic affiliation in Arandic languages. In I. Mushin, B. Baker, R. Gardner and M. Harvey (eds), Indigenous Language and Social Identity: Papers in Honour of Michael Walsh. Canberra. Australia: Pacific Linguistics, pp. 297-318.

TURPIN, M. and M. LAUGHREN. 2013. Edge effects in Warlpiri Yawulyu songs: Resyllabification, epenthesis, final vowel modification. Australian Journal of Linguistics 33(4): 399-425.

TURPIN, M. and A. ROSS. 2012. Kaytetye to English Dictionary. Alice Springs. Australia: IAD Press.

TURPIN, M. and A. ROSS (eds), 2013. Katie Kemarr Morton and Mary Kemarr Morton: Antarrengeny Awely. Alyawarr Women's Songs from Antarrengeny. Batchelor. Australia: Batchelor Press.

TURPIN, M.V., V. DOBSON, M. K. TURNER and A. N. ROSS. 2013. The spotted Nightjar calls when Dingo Pups are born: Ecological and social indicators in Central Australia. Journal of Ethnobiology 33(1): 7-32.

TURPIN, M., F. MEAKINS, and E. CHAROLA. In press. Wajarra. Songs of the Station. To be published by the University of Sydney Press.

VUILLEUMIER, P. 2009. Attention and emotion. In D. Sander and K. R. Scherer (eds), The Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 54-59.

WATSON, C. 2003. Piercing the Ground: Balgo Women's Image Making and Relationship to Country. Fremantle. Australia: Fremantle Arts Centre Press.

WILKINS, D. 1993. Linguistic evidence in support of a holistic approach to traditional ecological knowledge. In N. M. Williams, G. Baines and A. Brown-lee (eds), Traditional Ecological knowledge: Wisdom for Sustainable Development. Canberra, Australia: Centre for Resource and Environmental Studies. Australian National University, pp. 71-92.

YOUNG, D. 2006. Water as country on the Pitjantjatjara Yankunytjatjara Lands South Australia. Worldviews: Global Religions: Culture, and Ecology 10(2): 239-258.

--. 2011. Mutable things: Colours as material practice in the North West of South Australia. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 17: 356-376.

Myfany Turpin

University of Sydney

Nigel Fabb

University of Strathclyde
COPYRIGHT 2017 Blackwell Publishing Limited, a company of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2017 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Turpin, Myfany; Fabb, Nigel
Publication:Oceania
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:8AUST
Date:Jul 1, 2017
Words:11915
Previous Article:What is 'Kariera'? Detecting systems and overlap in Australian kinship using the AustKin database.
Next Article:The Empty Seashell: Witchcraft and Doubt on an Indonesian Island.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2020 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters