Printer Friendly

Bio-psycho-social correlates of the perceived crowding in different contexts.

A psychological perspective on crowding describes it as a situation of perceived high density that compromises privacy, the individual's control over desired interactions and the security offered by personal space (Hombrados-Mendieta, 2010). The concept has evolved over time. While initial approaches highlighted the negative effects of high density and information overload as the causes of environmental stress (Milgram, 1970), subsequent approaches shifted the focus to "control", leading to an approach which allows relationships between perceived density and control to be dealt with when assessing environmental settings (Rodin, Solomon, & Metcalf, 1978).

As Altman and Rogoff (1987) recognized in their reflection on "World Views in Psychology" regarding the scope of Environmental Psychology, the theoretical and methodological assumptions which manipulate and study complex realities as if they were schemes of simple relationships--a major trend in Environmental Psychology--have led to partial, unsatisfactory results to the problems that have been broached. This situation would justify the interest in and the effort to define and make operational wide-ranging approaches to the study of behavior and its contexts. Along these lines, the advances in integrated approaches to behavior and their contexts in Environmental Psychology (Wapner & Demick, 2002) have introduced broader perspectives that have been operationalized through peopleenvironment transactional proposals from a holistic viewpoint of their components and process (Werner, Brown, & Altman, 2002).

Most of empirical research on crowding has focused on the search for personal and situational variability. To a large extent, the categorization of environments according to the level of perceived control allows us to understand crowding as it is experienced in different settings, where the characteristics linked to these settings exert an influence on the perception of specific density conditions (Stokols, 1976). Nonetheless, as Santoyo and Anguera (1992) admit, most of these studies have been tackled from a "focal" perspective. These scholars suggest analyzing crowding as a "context" to complement focal approaches, taking into account the specific features of the spaces when effects are produced on the perception of density and the generation of particular behaviors. There is, however, very little literature on this contextual perspective of crowding. One exception is the paper by Bonnes, Bonaiuto and Ercolani (1991), who found that perceptions of crowding and housing satisfaction were related to and qualified by the sociophysical context. Gomez-Jacinto and Hombrados-Mendieta (2002) followed a similar conceptual approach. These scholars analyzed the interactive effects of household and community crowding on psychological stress and residential satisfaction, including social support as a mitigating factor for the negative effects caused by perceived excessive density.

The approach adopted as a general point of reference in this paper has assumed the need of broaching the multi-dimensional complexity involved in analyzing human behavior in general and spatial behavior in particular (Bell, Greene, Fisher, & Baum, 2001). This study's framework turns out to be particularly apt for the specific case of crowding, construed as a balance between the privacy one strives for and the privacy obtained in the midst of environmental expectations and conditions, which can lead to a possible perception of discomfort and/or loss of control over personal space (Altman, 1975). To the extent by which control over privacy and personal space are deemed unsatisfactory, the situation will therefore require recurring to adjustment mechanisms to balance out the perceived threatening conditions and the expectations of well-being sought in the person-environment adaptation. From this standpoint, the experience of crowding is conceptualized as an interdependent synthesis of environmental and social as well as cognitive and affective processes and components, as formulated in Environmental Psychology research by integrative multidimensional scheme approaches (Bell et al., 2001). Such an approach, as represented in Figure 1, could be tackled by combining factors from biological -refers to the inferences about psychological states based on physiological events, as considered by Environmental Psychophysiology (Parsons & Tassinary, 2002)-, psychological, and social dimensions, analyzing the relationships among these dimensions and perceived crowding and their variations across different environmental conditions.

The experience of crowding can thus be analyzed from a biopsycho-social perspective that contemplates both the affective response and the emotional reaction as the subject's first-level response (Ittelson, 1973) that interacts with cognitive assessment. The existing interdependence between the emotional and cognitive levels in environmental experience has been dealt with analytically through essential operating axes, like pleasure and arousal (Russell & Pratt, 1980), or security linked to "impact" and "control" factors (Corraliza, 1987), constituting a new more wide-ranging psychological level that interacts intensively with emotional (fear or anxiety) and motivational (security or comfort) factors. Furthermore, as can be seen in Figure 1, psychological dimension play a significant role in the environmental experience of crowding as a subjective representation of the density conditions perceived, where an individual's psychological aspects like personality, mood or perceived mental health can be related to the conditions perceived at an individual level (Aziraj & Ceranic, 2013). Hence, differential psychological factors constitute variability factors in an individual's response to the variation in environmental conditions through their changing expressions in the experience that is felt of environments, spatial behavior and the defense and regulation of personal space. Lastly, the meanings attributed to the environment's representations and images are to a great extent the outcome of the meanings acquired by social groups of reference (stereotypes, values, etc.) and reproduced in day-to-day social interactions connected with the use of space (Aragones, Amerigo, & Perez-Lopez, 2010), as well as in personal perceptions and images of the environment. Due to all of the above, the role played by other social variables which are also linked to representations of the environment and to spatial behavior patterns, such as socioeconomic conditions, educational level, residential habitat or cultural background (Ozdemir, 2011), can be highlighted in addition to classical variables like gender and age.

Although some research papers have been highlighted which broach a contextual conception of crowding, as far as the authors are aware there are no studies which empirically seek to establish a joint analysis of crowding's determinants from a biopsycho-social perspective, as described above. This paper aims to empirically develop such a theoretical proposal by exploring the joint impact on perceived crowding in different settings, through factors coming from a) the emotional response and affective meaning (BIO dimension); b) the perception of psychological well-being and personality traits (PSYCHO dimension) and c) sociodemographic characteristics (SOCIO dimension).

Method

Participants

A convenience sample was used, consisting of participants who were recruited by an invitation to participate in an online survey. It was distributed through a variety of dissemination channels (e-mail, social networks and personal contacts). Sevenhundred sixty-one adults from general Spanish population took part in the study. The average age was 40.9 years (SD =11.54). The distribution of participants by gender was 61.8% females. Concerning habitat, 45.5% of participants were essentially urban dwellers (from medium-sized and large cities and their suburbs).

Instrument

An online questionnaire structured around three sections was designed. Firstly, data on sociodemographic variables like residential habitat (large city, large city suburb, medium-sized city and rural environment), dwelling size ([m.sup.2]), age, gender, educational level and socioeconomic condition were collected.

The second section contained measures of perceived mood/ psychological well-being and personality traits. The Mental Health Scale (MH-5), a reduced five-item version obtained from the SF-36 Health questionnaire adapted by Alonso, Prieto and Anto (1995), was used for the former. This scale assesses the degree to which the person has experienced depression and anxiety symptoms in the last month on a six-point scale, ranging from always to never, with better mental health associated with a higher score. Personality was measured following the Big Five Model through an inventory adapted by Terracciano et al. (2005), comprised of 30 items, six for each personality trait, which are assessed through a five-point semantic differential scale.

The questionnaire ended by requesting the participant to assess four images representing four contexts (Figure 2) defined on the basis of two dimensions: density (high or low) and type of space (residential or functional). These images were chosen according to the results obtained by Tomas (2015).

Participants had to assess each of the four images based on the following variables:

--Affective assessment by means of the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) designed by Bradley and Lang (1994), where the participant indicated on a nine-point scale his/her level of pleasure (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal (calmaroused) after viewing the image.

--Additionally, an alternative affective assessment measure through the scale developed by Corraliza (1987) appeared only in the fourth image to avoid boredom. This instrument is comprised of 16 bipolar adjectives, on a five-point semantic differential scale, and measures the environmental meaning associated with a place through four dimensions: pleasure, arousal, impact, and control.

--Perceived crowding by means of the single-item crowding measure proposed by Vaske and Shelby (2008). The item assesses the sensation of crowding as perceived social density on a nine-point scale, ranging from comfort to discomfort.

The order in which the four images were shown changed depending on a randomized sequence.

Procedure

The survey was accessible between November 2012 and March 2013. The data obtained were analyzed in a comprehensive way, relating the perceived crowding in each of the four contexts with three group of explanatory variables: the biological (BIO: affective assessment), psychological (PSYCHO: psychological well-being and personality traits) and social (SOCIO: age, gender, educational level, socioeconomic conditions, residential habitat and dwelling size) dimensions. These analyses were conducted with hierarchical regressions in which the predictive variables were all those which made up the three aforementioned dimensions. Each of these composed a block and were included in the regression hierarchically in order to analyze each dimension's joint contribution. The perceived crowding in each context was the criterion variable. Statistical analysis was carried out using IBM[R] SPSS[R] Statistics 19.0 and R 3.1.1.

Results

Reliability analysis and descriptive statistics

Initially, the reliability of the psychological well-being and the five personality traits scales was assessed by calculating the ordinal alpha, as recommended by Gadermann, Guhn and Zumbo (2012) for scales with two to seven response options. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of each one of them and the ordinal alpha. On the one hand, high average scores can be observed for psychological well-being, as well as for the traits of conscientiousness, agreeableness, extraversion and openness to experience. On the other hand, intermediate average scores were found for neuroticism. The reliability analysis showed appropriate values above or very close to .7, with the exception of the scale which measures the openness to experience, whose ordinal alpha turned out to be .64.

The reliability of the biological dimension, which corresponds to the affective variables obtained through the scale developed by Corraliza (1987), was also analyzed. As mentioned in the instrument's description, this scale was only applied to the image shown fourth and last. Table 2 shows the reliability analysis and descriptive statistics of this scale. Taking into account the different contexts, the ordinal alpha shows a high reliability for pleasure and control, adequate values for impact (except in the high-density residential context with an ordinal alpha of .54) and very low reliability for arousal (which in no context reaches the value of .6). A decision was therefore taken to disregard the latter variable in successive analyses, since an alternative measure from the SAM was available.

Table 2 also shows the descriptive statistics of the affective assessment obtained through the SAM and the perceived crowding. Data show: high values for arousal and low values for pleasure in high-density residential and functional contexts; and high values for pleasure and relatively low values for arousal in low-density residential and functional contexts. In consonance with these results, the descriptive statistics for the measure of perceived crowding show high values in high-density residential and functional contexts, and low values in low-density residential contexts.

Analysis of the biological, psychological and social dimensions of crowding

Before proceeding with the analysis of the impact of the biological, psychological and social dimensions on perceived crowding in each of the four contexts, any nominal or ordinal variables having more than two categories were converted into dummy variables with a value equivalent to 1 for the case of subjects: a) with university studies; b) who were in paid employment (workers); and c) who lived in a dwelling measuring more than 100 [m.sup.2].

Table 3 shows the four regression analyses in which the three dimensions were hierarchically included in each of the equations in ordered blocks. It is thus possible to assess the increase in [R.sup.2] resulting from the inclusion of each of the dimensions under study. We underline that pleasure as measured through the SAM had high collinearity, a logical circumstance if it is taken into account that a measure of pleasure proposed by Corraliza (1987) was also available. That is why it was decided to dispense with "Pleasure - SAM", as the alternative measure turned out to have high internal consistency. Furthermore, it also was also necessary to exclude independent variables from one or several regressions on a specific basis when the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) indicated the existence of collinearity.

As can be observed in Table 3, the explained variance percentage ranged from 36% in the case of the crowding perceived in a lowdensity functional context to 15% in a high-density functional context. By conducting a segmented study on each of the dimensions, it was confirmed that the biological dimension is the one which best explains a greater proportion of the variance in the crowding perceived in three of the four contexts under study (HDR, HDF and LDF). In the case of the LDR, the psychological dimension takes on greater importance, explaining 18% of the perceived crowding's variance within this context. As for the social dimension, the increase in [R.sup.2] resulting from taking into consideration this kind of variables only turned out to be significant for residential contexts (HDR and LDR) and not for functional contexts (HDF and LDF).

Discussion

Results reveal that the factors that best explain perceived crowding are those included in the BIO, followed by SOCIO and PSYCHO dimensions, though the order of the latter depends on the environmental context under analysis. More specifically, the variables in the BIO dimension are the ones which best explain the variation in crowding in functional and in high-density residential contexts. On another hand, variables in PSYCHO dimension turned out to explain perceived crowding best in the case of the-low density residential context.

There was a direct relationship between perceived crowding and arousal, and an inverse relationship with pleasantness in the BIO dimension. This outcome is consistent with the results obtained by Tomas, Amerigo and Aragones (2016), who revealed that low-density spaces generated more pleasant feelings while high-density spaces generated a greater degree of arousal. A negative effect of control on crowding was also found in low-density functional contexts. This is consistent with the description of secondary spaces outlined by Stokols (1976), which are characterized by transient, anonymous and discontinuous meetings where people usually put up barriers to discourage interactions with strangers.

As regards the variables analyzed in the PSYCHO dimension, both perceived well-being as well as personality traits only turned out to be significant in low-density contexts. The negative association between psychological well-being and crowding was congruent. On another hand, openness to experience was associated with less crowding both in functional and residential contexts, whereas extroversion was associated with greater perceived crowding in residential contexts, contrary to what would be expected. The Big Five Model in residential contexts has been studied in connection with the inference of traits through decoration by Aragones et al. (2010). Nevertheless, according to these authors (Perez-Lopez, Aragones, & Amerigo, 2013), it seems that more consistent results could be obtained with more comprehensive models linked to social cognition, such as the Stereotype Content Model (SCM). The design of valid reliable instruments adapted to the Spanish context to measure the SCM's dimensions of competence and warmth--like the one recently drawn up by Aragones, Poggio, Sevillano, Perez-Lopez and Sanchez-Bernardos (2015)--will allow future research to obtain findings more consistent than the ones contained herein.

As for the SOCIO dimension, this had no significant impact on functional contexts. In residential environments, however, males and workers showed less tolerance to crowding in high-density conditions, and university graduates showed greater tolerance in low-density conditions. The findings on gender contrast with those found by Yildirim and Akalin-Baskaya (2007) in public spaces regarding the handling of personal space by males, who showed a greater tolerance to intrusion. Similarly, these findings do not coincide with the results obtained by Tomas et al. (2016), who found that almost all the average scores for the arousal elicited by different high and low-density environments were greater among women. Nonetheless, these studies did not analyze the different variables' intervention on environmental experience jointly. Future research on this field should look into the moderating effect of gender on crowding in different settings.

To sum up, the results of this study have shown that there is a more homogenous response pattern to crowding in functional settings that is less dependent on individual differences. It is essentially defined by an affective response and marked by the need to control these spaces in low-density contexts, and the experience of high arousal and low pleasantness in high-density settings. These results are consistent with classic studies conducted on behavior in public spaces taking different approaches (Goffman, 1971; Hall, 1966; Morris, 1979). Nonetheless, in the case of residential environments, different explanatory variables for crowding appear which are linked to individual differences and associated with these spaces' more personal character and identity. The response to the experience of the environment in residential spaces therefore shows a greater range of variables involved in the experience and perception of and adaptation to these environments, giving greater weight to individual differences, thereby supporting the notion of these spaces' greater complexity, in so far as they are psychologically adaptive environments (Rollero, 2013; Tomas et al., 2016).

These results reveal that relevant contributions can be deduced when spaces to optimize people-environment interactions are designed. For example, an interesting result is the higher explained variance of crowding obtained in low-density areas (especially functional) compared to high-density. This finding enables designers and architects realize that low-density spaces will not necessarily be perceived as being not very crowded. According to the results, control over space seems to be a determining factor in the design of functional low-density environments with regard to crowding. Although a particular context has been used here--the inside of a train carriage (Figure 2)-, similar results have as a matter of fact been found for other functional contexts. According to Molina, Melendez and Navarro (2008), the institutionalized elderly felt a lower degree of control over the environment than the non-institutionalized, which results in less subjective well-being.

As Werner et al. (2002) points out, the holistic approach of a transactional world view is often needed in Environmental Psychology. Although the conceptual and methodological developments presented in this study are still far from reaching this goal, the analysis of the biological, psychological and social dimensions of the experience of crowding through the results obtained offers an interpretation of people-environment interaction schemes in an effort to move forward in an integrated analysis of environmental experience. The difficulty encountered in conducting a joint causal analysis of the multiple measures taken into account has to be considered. Any improvement along these lines, for example using structural equation modeling to assess relationships between BIO, SOCIO and PSYCHO dimensions, would therefore constitute an advance in the multidimensional comprehension of perceived crowding. Another limitation connected to the research procedure has to do with the fact that all the information obtained was from a self-administered survey, including the assessments made of virtual environments. It is believed that such an experience would elicit a response similar to the one that would probably be associated with the physical experience of the environments themselves. The constraints arising from the use of an indirect record for the assessment of the experience of crowding and from the application of a technique using images, as opposed to real environments, leaves the research's design open to questioning. The role played by other relevant variables associated with confronting environmental stress, such as social support (Gomez-Jacinto & Hombrados-Mendieta, 2002) or place attachment (Magalhaes & Calheiros, 2015), also remains open for future research.

doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.336

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

References

Alonso, J., Prieto, L., & Anto, J. M. (1995). La version espanola del SF36 Health Survey (Cuestionario de Salud SF-36): un instrumento para la medida de los resultados clinicos [The Spanish version of the SF36 Health Survey: An instrument for measuring clinical outcomes]. Medicina Clinica, 104, 771-776.

Altman, I. (1975). The environment and social behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Altman, I., & Rogoff, B. (1987). World-views in psychology: Trait, interactional, organismic and transactional perspectives. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 7-40). New York: Wiley.

Aragones, J. I., Amerigo, M., & Perez-Lopez, R. (2010). Perception of personal identity at home. Psicothema, 22, 872-879.

Aragones, J. I., Poggio, L., Sevillano, V., Perez-Lopez, R., & SanchezBernardos, M. L. (2015). Measuring warmth and competence at intergroup, interpersonal and individual levels. Revista de Psicologia Social, 30, 407-438.

Aziraj, V., & Ceramic, S. (2013). Differences in the size of personal space between persons with anxious and persons with psychotic disorders. Psychiatria Danubina, 25, 163-169.

Bell, P. A., Greene, T. C., Fisher, J. D., & Baum, A. (2001). Environmental psychology. Orlando, FL: Hartcourt College Publishers.

Bonnes, M., Bonaiuto, M., & Ercolani, A. P. (1991). Crowding and residential satisfaction in the urban environment: A contextual approach. Environment and Behavior, 23, 531-552.

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The SelfAssessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49-59.

Corraliza, J. A. (1987). La experiencia del ambiente. Percepcion y significado del medio construido [The environment experience. Perception and meaning of the built environment]. Madrid: Tecnos.

Gadermann, A. M., Guhn, M., & Zumbo, B. D. (2012). Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data: A conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 17(3). Retrieved from: http://pareonline.net/pdf/v17n3.pdf

Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in public: Microstudies of the public order. New York: Basic Books.

Gomez-Jacinto, L., & Hombrados-Mendieta, I. (2002). Multiple effects of community and household crowding. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 233-246.

Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday.

Hombrados-Mendieta, I. (2010). Hacinamiento [Crowding]. In J. I. Aragones & M. Amerigo (Eds.), Psicologia ambiental (pp. 141-162). Madrid: Piramide.

Ittelson, W. H. (1973). Environment and cognition. New York: Seminar Press.

Magalhaes, E., & Calheiros, M. M. (2015). Psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of place attachment scale for youth in residential care. Psicothema, 27, 65-73.

Milgram, S. (1970). The experience of living in cities. Science, 167, 1461-1468.

Molina, C., Melendez, J. C., & Navarro, E. (2008). Bienestar y calidad de vida en ancianos institucionalizados y no institucionalizados [Well-being and life quality among old people living and not living in institutions for the elderly]. Anales de Psicologia, 24, 312-319.

Morris, D. (1979). Manwatching: A field guide to human behavior. New York: Abrams.

Ozdemir, M. (2011). Marketing management in construction industry: Cultural differences in consumers' housing choice. Problems of Management in the 21st Century, 1. Retrieved from http://www. scientiasocialis.lt/pmc/view/biblio/year/2011/volume/1

Parsons, R., & Tassinary, L G. (2002). Environmental psychophysiology. In R. B. Bechtel & A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 172-190). New York: Wiley.

Perez-Lopez, R., Aragones, J. I., & Amerigo, M. (2013). Thin slices of competence and warmth via personalized primary spaces. Psyecology, 4, 267-286.

Rodin, J., Solomon, S. K., & Metcalf, J. (1978). Role of control in mediating perceptions of density. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 988-999.

Rollero, C. (2013). The town in my mind: How place attachment and identification are linked to place perception. Estudios de Psicologia, 34, 309-314.

Russell, J. A., & Pratt, G (1980). A description of the affective quality attributed to environments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 311-322.

Santoyo, C., & Anguera, M. T. (1992). El hacinamiento como contexto: estrategias metodologicas para su analisis [Crowding as context: Analytical and methodological strategies]. Psicothema, 4, 551-569.

Stokols, D. (1976). The experience of crowding in primary and secondary environments. Environment and Behavior, 8, 49-86.

Terracciano, A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Adam, N., Adamovova, L., Ahn, C-k., Ahn, H.-n., McCrae, R. R. (2005). National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures. Science, 310, 96100.

Tomas, R. (2015). Hacinamiento, estres ambiental y espacio personal: una aproximacion bio-psico-social al comportamiento espacial en humanos [Crowding, environmental stress and personal space: A bio-psico-social approach to human spatial behavior]. Retrieved from https://ruidera.uclm.es/xmlui/handle/10578/7766

Tomas, R., Amerigo, M., & Aragones, J. I. (2016). Affective assessment of high and low-density residential and functional environments. Anales de Psicologia. doi: 10.6018/analesps.32.3.220941

Vaske, J. J., & Shelby, L. B. (2008). Crowding as a descriptive indicator and an evaluative standard: Results from 30 years of research. Leisure Sciences, 30, 111-126.

Wapner, S., & Demick, J. (2002). The increasing contexts of context in the study of environment behavior relations. In R. B. Bechtel & A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 3-14). New York: Wiley.

Werner, C. M., Brown, B. B., & Altman, I. (2002). Transactionally oriented research: Examples and strategies. In R. B. Bechtel & A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 203-221). New York: Wiley.

Yildirim, K., & Akalin-Baskaya, A. (2007). Perceived crowding in a cafe/ restaurant with different seating densities. Building and Environment, 42, 3410-3417.

Rafael Tomas, Maria Amerigo and Juan A. Garcia

Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha

Received: December 20, 2015 * Accepted: June 10, 2016

Corresponding author: Maria Amerigo Cuervo-Arango

Facultad de Humanidades de Toledo

Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha

45071 Toledo (Spain)

e-mail: Maria.Amerigo@uclm.es

Table 1
Reliability analysis and descriptive statistics of the measures in
PSYCHO dimension

       Construct            N     M      SD    Minimum   Maximum

Psychological well-being   761   4.38   0.81    1.20      6.00
Neuroticism                761   2.35   0.60    2.00      4.33
Openness to experience     761   3.71   0.61    1.50      5.00
Conscientiousness          761   3.96   0.57    2.17      5.00
Extraversion               761   3.71   0.61    1.33      5.00
Agreeableness              761   3.91   0.53    2.00      5.00

                             N     Ordinal
       Construct           items    alpha

Psychological well-being     5       .85
Neuroticism                  6       .68
Openness to experience       6       .64
Conscientiousness            6       .77
Extraversion                 6       .70
Agreeableness                6       .68

Table 2
Reliability analysis and descriptive statistics of the measures
in BIO dimension and perceived crowding

    Construct/        N     M      SD    Minimum
     Context

Arousal--SAM
  HDR                761   4.64   2.81      1
  LDR                761   3.06   2.09      1
  HDF                761   5.63   2.82      1
  LDF                761   2.57   1.57      1

Pleasure--SAM
  HDR                761   1.87   1.46      1
  LDR                761   7.56   1.52      1
  HDF                761   1.66   1.36      1
  LDF                761   5.57   1.77      1

Arousal
  HDR                174   4.09   0.69    1.33
  LDR                142   2.48   0.49    1.00
  HDF                248   4.39   0.54    2.33
  LDF                197   2.70   0.55    1.00

Pleasure
  HDR                174   1.58   0.73    1.00
  LDR                142   4.49   0.53    3.00
  HDF                248   1.26   0.43    1.00
  LDF                197   3.53   0.74    1.00

Control
  HDR                174   1.76   0.74    1.00
  LDR                142   4.16   0.58    1.75
  HDF                248   1.61   0.63    1.00
  LDF                197   3.80   0.70    2.00

Impact
  HDR                174   3.24   0.85    1.00
  LDR                142   3.68   0.63    2.00
  HDF                248   2.95   0.91    1.00
  LDF                197   3.15   0.43    2.00

Perceived crowding
  HDR                761   7.55   2.11      1
  LDR                761   1.79   1.56      1
  HDF                761   8.20   1.67      1
  LDF                761   2.21   1.50      1

    Construct/       Maximum     N     Ordinal
     Context                   Items    alpha

Arousal--SAM
  HDR                   9        1      n.a.
  LDR                   9        1      n.a.
  HDF                   9        1      n.a.
  LDF                   9        1      n.a.

Pleasure--SAM
  HDR                   9        1      n.a.
  LDR                   9        1      n.a.
  HDF                   9        1      n.a.
  LDF                   9        1      n.a.

Arousal
  HDR                 5.00       4       .59
  LDR                 3.67       4       .52
  HDF                 5.00       4       .34
  LDF                 4.33       4       .41

Pleasure
  HDR                 5.00       4       .91
  LDR                 5.00       4       .85
  HDF                 3.50       4       .87
  LDF                 5.00       4       .88

Control
  HDR                 5.00       4       .83
  LDR                 5.00       4       .68
  HDF                 4.00       4       .81
  LDF                 5.00       4       .78

Impact
  HDR                 5.00       4       .54
  LDR                 5.00       4       .73
  HDF                 5.00       4       .63
  LDF                 4.75       4       .68

Perceived crowding
  HDR                   9        1      n.a.
  LDR                   9        1      n.a.
  HDF                   9        1      n.a.
  LDF                   9        1      n.a.

Note: HDR=High-Density Residential; LDR=Low-Density Residential;
HDF=High-Density Functional; LDF=Low-Density Functional;
n.a.: not applicable

Table 3
Hierarchical regression analyses for the perceived crowding in
high/low-density residential/functional contexts

                                            Perceived crowding

     Block/Explanatory variable           HDR            LDR

Block 1. Biological dimension
Arousal--SAM                              .07            .06
Pleasure                               - 40 ***         -.11
Control                                                  .13
Impact                                   -.02            .03
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                          .20            .05
F change                               10.80 ***        1.44

Block 2. Psychological dimension
Psychological well-being                  .09          -.25 *
Neuroticism                               .07
Openness to experience                    .04      -.20 ([dagger])
Conscientiousness                        -.11           -.14
Extraversion                             -.01           .27 *
Agreeableness                             .03           -.13
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                          .03            .18
F change                                 0.94         5.20 ***

Block 3. Social dimension
Age                                                      .01
Gender (d) (1=male)                      .21 *           .00
Education (d) (1=university studies)     -.03         -.30 ***
Socioeconomic condition (1=worker)       .18 *           .08
Residential habitat (d)
  (1=rural environment)                  -.10           -.09
Dwelling size
  (1=more than 100 [m.sup.2])             .02           -.07
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                          .08            .10
F change                                2.93 *         2.49 *
[R.sup.2]                                 .31            .33
F                                      3.96 ***       3.35 ***

                                         Perceived crowding

     Block/Explanatory variable          HDF            LDF

Block 1. Biological dimension
Arousal--SAM                           .25 ***         .17 *
Pleasure                                -.14 *         -.12
Control                                              -.37 ***
Impact                                   .06            .02
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                         .11            .28
F change                               8.91 ***      14.25 ***

Block 2. Psychological dimension
Psychological well-being                 .03           -.04
Neuroticism
Openness to experience                   .02      -.14 ([dagger])
Conscientiousness                        -.02          -.03
Extraversion                             .05            .01
Agreeableness                            .03           -.05
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                         .01            .05
F change                                 0.41     2.04 ([dagger])

Block 3. Social dimension
Age                                      .12           -.15
Gender (d) (1=male)                      .08            .08
Education (d) (1=university studies)     .03           -.02
Socioeconomic condition (1=worker)       -.13
Residential habitat (d)
  (1=rural environment)                  .07            .06
Dwelling size
  (1=more than 100 [m.sup.2])            -.09          -.03
[DELTA][R.sup.2]                         .03            .03
F change                                 1.15          1.28
[R.sup.2]                                .15            .36
F                                       2.53 *       5.45 ***

Note: HDR=High-Density Residential; LDR=Low-Density Residential;
HDF=High-Density Functional; LDF=Low-Density Functional; (d) dummy
variable. Missing variables in the regression models were excluded
due to collinearity

*** p<.001; ** p<.01; * p<.05; ([dagger]) p<.1
COPYRIGHT 2016 Colegio Oficial De Psicologos Del Principado De Asturias
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2016 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:texto en ingles
Author:Tomas, Rafael; Amerigo, Maria; Garcia, Juan A.
Publication:Psicothema
Date:Oct 1, 2016
Words:5131
Previous Article:Stress in Spanish police force depending on occupational rank, sex, age and work-shift.
Next Article:A predictive study of antecedent variables of workaholism.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2021 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters |