Printer Friendly

Behavioral characterization of attractin, a water-borne peptide pheromone in the genus Aplysia.

Introduction

Chemical communication is the most ancient form of communication and is used by most, if not all, animals examined. The organisms include, for example, ciliated protozoans (Luporini et al., 1995), yeast (Kodama et al., 2003), insects (Monsma and Wolfner, 1988; Roelofs et al., 2002; Saudan et al., 2002), molluscs (Painter et al., 1998), worms (Ram et al., 1999), fish (Li et al., 2002), amphibians (Kikuyama et al., 1995; Rollmann et al., 1999; Wabnitz et al., 1999), rodents (Stowers et al., 2002; Novotny, 2003) and humans (Savic et al., 2001.). The number of pheromones characterized in each species depends, at least in part, on the chemical nature of the pheromones and on whether the pheromones are water-borne.

Opisthobranch molluscs of the genus Aplysia are simultaneous hermaphrodites that do not normally fertilize their own eggs. Field studies (Kupfermann and Carew, 1974; Audesirk, 1979; Susswein et al., 1983, 1984) have shown that they are solitary animals that move into breeding aggregations during the reproductive season. The aggregations typically contain both mating and egg-laying animals and are associated with masses of recently deposited egg cordons, often deposited one on top of another. Most of the egg-laying animals mate simultaneously as females even though mating does not cause reflex ovulation (Blankenship et al., 1983), suggesting that egg laying precedes mating in the aggregation and that egg laying may release pheromones that establish and maintain the aggregation.

Similar observations have been made in the laboratory when animals were not individually caged (Audesirk, 1979; Blankenship et al., 1983; Susswein et al., 1983, 1984), and behavioral studies have shown that egg-laying animals with cordons are more attractive thin3 sexually mature but non-laying conspecifics (Aspey and Blankenship, 1976; Jahan-Parwar, 1976; Audesirk, 1977; Painter et al., 1989). T-maze assays show that at least some of the attractants derive from the egg cordon and are waterborne: (1) recent egg-layers without egg cordons are no more attractive than non-laying conspecifics; (2) recently deposited egg cordons are attractive, with or without a non-laying conspecific, but sham egg cordons are not; and (3) both recently deposited egg cordons and their eluates increase the attractiveness of non-laying conspecifics when placed in the surrounding seawater (Painter et al., 1991; Painter, 1992).

One of the water-borne pheromonal attractants has been isolated from eluates of the egg cordon and characterized. Nmned attractin, it is a 58-residue peptide that has six cysteines that form three intramolecular disulfide bonds (Fig. 1; Painter et al., 1998; Schein et al., 2001). Attractin was isolated from a Pacific Coast species (A. californica) and bioassayed in a species from the Gulf of Mexico (A. brasiliana). This was done because individuals of A. californica tend to crawl out of T-maze chambers before they are exposed to the stimulus. A. californica attractin was attractive to A. brasiliana and produced behaviors that were suggestive of mating (Painter et al., 1998), but these behaviors were not further analyzed. The amount of attractin that was attractive to conspecifics and induced the potential mating behaviors (1-10 pmol in 6 1 artificial seawater) was in the range of concentrations normally observed with pheromones, demonstrating that attractin has pheromonal activity.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

There is no geographical overlap between the distributions of the two species, suggesting that attractin or an attractin-related peptide is a pheromonal attractant in A. brasiliana. A peptide was subsequently isolated from the A. brasiliana albumen gland and sequenced. It is 58 amino acids in length and differs from A. californica attractin at only 3 amino acids (Fig. 1; Painter et al., 2000). It is deposited on the egg cordon and elutes into the seawater following deposition It could thus serve a pheromonal function in A. brasiliana, but its pheromonal activities have yet to be tested.

In the present study, behavioral assays were used to better characterize the attraction and to examine whether mating is induced. The current T-maze assays showed that attractin works as part of a bouquet of water-borne odors: the peptide is attractive only when individuals of A. brasiliana or A. californica are part of the stimulus. The animal does not need to be a conspecific, perhaps explaining why multiple species of Aplvsia may be associated with one aggregation--for example, A. vaccaria with A. californica aggregations (Kupfermann and Carew, 1974; Pennings, 1991); A. californica with A. vaccaria aggregations (S. LePage, Marine Research and Educational Products (M-REP), pers. comm.): and A. depilans with A. fasciata aggregations (Achituv and Susswein, 1985). Recombinant attractin was also tested for two reasons: (1) to see whether N-glycosylation at [Asn.sup.8] is necessary for attraction (native attractin is glycosylated and recombinant attractin is not); and (2) to see whether the two are equally attractive, so that recombinant attractin could be used in 3D nuclear magnetic resonance solution structure studies (Garimella et al., 2003) and for future studies of mechanism of action at the receptor level.

A series of mating assays was performed because behaviors in earlier T-maze assays suggested that both the stimulus and test animals wanted to mate (Painter et al., 1998). The assays showed that attractin reduces the latency to mating at concentrations consistent with a pheromone. Attractin also reduces the latency to hermaphroditic mating and doubles the number of pairs mating as hermaphrodites. This effect may result from attractin stimulating both animals to mate as males and would be consistent with behaviors seen in earlier T-maze assays (Painter et al., 1998). These results suggest that attractin, acting in an aggregation where there are more animals, could be at least partially responsible for the copulatory chains and rings that have been observed. Recombinant attractin also induces mating, both one-way and as a hermaphrodite, showing that N-glycosylation is not required for the induction of either type of mating. The attraction and mating data demonstrate that attractin may contribute to the establishment and maintenance of breeding aggregations, and to successful reproduction.

Materials, Methods, and Results

Animals

Specimens of Aplysia brasiliana (Rang), ranging in weight from 100 to 500 g, were collected from South Padre Island, Texas, and were used in experiments between June and September. A. brasiliana was used as the experimental animal in the T-maze and mating experiments because it is more reproductively active than A. californica (see fig. 2 in Painter et al., 1998), does not crawl out of T-mazes, makes fewer false choices, and can be collected in large numbers from the south Texas coast during the reproductive season. Previous T-maze assays (Painter et al., 1998) showed that an individual of A. brasiliana is attracted to a non-laying conspecific and displays behaviors suggestive of mating when 10 pmol of attractin is placed in the adjacent artificial seawater, even though attractin is a product of the A. californica albumen gland.

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

The animals were housed in individual plastic cages in one of five aquaria containing recirculating artificial seawater (ASW; Instant Ocean Marine Salt, Longhorn Pet Supply, Houston, Texas). Water was maintained at room temperature (20[degrees] [+ or -] 2[degrees]C); the salinity ranged from 30 to 32 ppt. A 14:10 light:dark cycle was maintained in the aquarium facility, with the light period starting at 0600. Animals were fed dried laver in the late afternoon (1600-1800) after experiments were completed.

Egg-laying activity was checked twice every day (0800-0900, 1600-1800), egg-laying activity was recorded, and egg cordons were removed. All animals used in assays were sexually mature, as defined by the ability to lay eggs spontaneously or in response to injection of atrial gland extracts (made as described in Painter et al., 1991).

Specimens of A. californica (Cooper) were obtained from Alacrity Marine Biological Services (Redondo Beach, California) and M-REP (Escondido, California). They were maintained as described above, except that the water temperature was 14[degrees] [+ or -] 2[degrees]C. This species was used as a stimulus animal in one set of T-maze assays and as the source of albumen glands for purification of native attractin.

Purification of native and recombinant attractin

Procedures. Attractin from the albumen gland of A. californica was purified by analytical C18 reversed-phase high performance liquid chromatography (RP-HPLC) as previously described (Painter et al., 1998). To prepare recombinant attractin, the A. californica albumen-gland attractin cDNA (Fan et al., 1997) was subcloned into the baculovirus expression vector pFastBac 1, and recombinant virus was generated using the Bac-to-Bac Baculovirus Expression System (Invitrogen). Attractin was expressed in Sf9 insect cells grown at 27-28[degrees]C in Sf-900 II serum-free medium (Invitrogen).

Expressing Sf9 cells were centrifuged, and the pellet was resuspended in 20 ml of ice-cold 0.1% heptafluorobutyric acid (HFBA) and sonicated. The resulting lysates were purified on C 18 Sep-Pak Vac cartridges (5 g; Waters Corp.) that were pretreated with 10 ml of 100% acetonitrile (C[H.sub.3]CN) containing 0.1% HFBA and rinsed with 20 ml of 0.1% HFBA. The peptides were loaded, eluted with 15 ml of 50% C[H.sub.3]CN containing 0.1% HFBA, and lyophilized. The lyophilizate was resuspended in 2.5 ml of 0.1% HFBA and applied to a Vydac analytical C18 RP-HPLC column (4.6 x 250 mm).

The column was eluted with a two-step linear gradient of 0.1% HFBA in water and 100% C[H.sub.3]CN containing 0.1% HFBA. The first step was 0%-10% C[H.sub.3]CN in 5 rain, followed by a shallower gradient from 10% to 34% C[H.sub.3]CN in 85 rain. The column eluate was monitored at 215 nm, and 1-min (1 ml) fractions were collected. The attractin-containing fractions were combined, lyophilized, and repurified by Vydac C18 RP-HPLC. The same gradient conditions were used as described above, except that 0.1% trifluoroacetic acid was the counterion.

Results. The RP-HPLC peak fractions containing A. californica recombinant attractin, identifed by comparison to the elution time of native attractin, were characterized by amino acid compositional and microsequence analyses; the 58-residue peptide sequence was identical to A. californica albumen-gland attractin except that, according to matrix-assisted laser desorption/ionization mass spectrometry, the native peptide is N-glycosylated at [Asn.sup.8] and the recombinant peptide is not.

Pheromonal attraction

Procedures. The T-maze, and its associated cages, is illustrated in Figure 2. Before each assay, 6 1 of ASW was put into the maze; the ASW was stationary during experiments. To minimize the amount of stress experienced by the animals during transfer to the maze, the ASW was similar in temperature and salinity to that in the aquarium from which the animals were taken. The ASW placed in the maze had not previously contacted A. californica or A. brasiliana, because there are animal-derived factors that make a non-laying conspecific attractive (Painter et al., 1991).

A non-laying conspecific was placed in one of the stimulus cages and a potential attractant added to the adjacent ASW; this is the stimulus animal. After 5 min, a non-laying animal, known as the test animal, was placed in the base of the maze and watched for up to 20 rain. In most cases, the test animal moved directly to the top of the maze and exhibited one of two patterns of behavior. (1) It stopped, moved its head from side to side, then either moved into one arm or returned to the base of the maze and remained there. (2) It swam around in the maze, often visiting both cages before deciding where to stop. A response was considered to be positive if the test animal traveled to the stimulus within 20 min, and then maintained contact with the stimulus cage for 5 min. It was negative if the test animal traveled to the cage in the opposite arm and maintained contact for 5 min. The response was considered to be no choice if the test animal did neither. Ten assays were performed for every potential attractant, and the attractant was alternated between arms in consecutive assays. Statistical significance was assessed by chi-square analysis. In each case, test animals were choosing between a stimulus in one arm and no stimulus in the other.

Animals for each assay were selected on the basis of three criteria. First, the animals must have been sexually mature but not have laid eggs or been used in a bioassay during the preceding 24 h. Second, the test animal must not have been exposed previously to the fraction being tested. Third, stimulus and test animals must have been housed in the same aquarium (Painter et al., 1998). An exception was made to the third criterion in one set of assays, when A. californica was used as the stimulus animal. A. brasiliana was always used as the test animal (Painter et al., 1998).

Several series of experiments were performed. The first compared the attractiveness of a non-laying specimen of A. brasiliana with 1 pmol of either native or recombinant attractin in the adjacent ASW to a non-laying conspecific with nothing added. We were asking several questions. Are smaller amounts of native attractin attractive? Is recombinant attractin as attractive? Would it be feasible to use recombinant attractin in future behavioral, molecular, or 3D structural studies? The second series examined whether a non-laying conspecific was needed for 1 pmol attractin to be attractive. We were asking: does attractin function alone or as part of a "bouquet of scents," as other pheromones do in many systems? The third series examined whether the non-laying animal must be a conspecific. We were asking: could the presence of multiple species at one breeding aggregation be due, partially or completely, to attractin? If animal-derived factors are necessary, do they differ among species?

Results. The results of the experiments comparing the attractiveness of native and recombinant attractin are shown in Figure 3. In the negative control (non-laying conspecific with nothing placed in the adjacent ASW), two animals (20%) traveled to the right arm and remained, two (20%) traveled to the left arm and remained, and six (60%) did neither. Of the four animals making a choice, only two went to the stimulus animal, one of which was in the right arm and the other of which was in the left arm of the maze. These bioassays verify that there is no directional bias in the maze and establish chance levels of attraction at two animals.

[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]

The response pattern changed when 1 pmol of either native or recombinant attractin was placed in the seawater adjacent to the stimulus animal (Fig. 3): 9 of 10 animals (90%) were attracted to recombinant attractin, and 8 of 10 animals (80%) were attracted to native attractin; in both cases, fewer animals went to the opposite arm and fewer failed to make a choice. The response patterns for each differed significantly from that for a non-laying conspecific alone [recombinant: [chi square](2) = 13.75; P < 0.005; native: [chi square](2) = 10.44, 0.05 < P < 0.1], but did not differ significantly from each other [[chi square](2) = 2.1, 0.25 < P < 0.5].

The results of the experiment examining whether an animal is needed for attraction arc shown in Figure 3. When 1 pmol recombinant attractin was placed in the seawater without a stimulus animal, 3 of 10 animals (30%) were attracted to recombinant attractin, two animals (20%) went to the opposite arm, and five animals (50%) did neither (Fig. 3). The response pattern to 1 pmol recombinant attractin alone differed significantly from that to 1 pmol recombinant attractin with an animal [chi square](2) = 6.00; 0.025 < P < 0.05], but did not differ from that to a non-laying conspecific alone [chi square](2) = 0.277; 0.95 < P < 0.9751.

Animals do not release attractin unless they are laying eggs; therefore, the combined odor of a non-laying animal and attractin produces a qualitatively different stimulus from attractin alone. The data confirm that attractin functions as part of a bouquet of scents and led us to ask, Does the animal-derived pheromone have to come from a conspecific or can it come from a different species of Aplysia, perhaps accounting for the presence of multiple species at an aggregation? This would also be consistent with reports of multiple species showing up at one aggregation in the field.

The results of experiments examining whether the stimulus animal needs to be a conspeeific in order for attractin to be attractive are shown in Figure 3. When 1 pmol of recombinant attractin was placed in the seawater adjacent to A. brasiliana, 8 of 10 A. brasiliana (80%) were attracted to the non-laying conspecific. When 1 pmol of recombinant attractin was placed in the seawater adjacent to A. californica, 7 of 10 A. brasiliana (70%) were attracted to the non-laying A. californica. The response patterns for the two species did not differ significantly from each other [[chi square](2) = 0.265; 0.75 < P < 0.9], but each differed significantly from that for a non-laying A. brasiliana alone [A. brasiliana, [chi square](2) = 10.44; 0.005 < P < 0.01; A. californica, [[chi square](2) = 7.50; 0.005 < P < 0.01].

Pheromonal induction of mating activity

Procedures. As in the T-maze bioassays, three criteria were used to select animals for each experiment. First, the animals must have been sexually mature but not have laid eggs or been used in a bioassay during the previous 24 h. Second, the animals must not have been exposed previously to the fraction being tested or have been paired with the same animal twice. Third, both animals must have been housed in the same aquarium (Painter et al., 1998).

Each assay was performed in 3 1 of aerated ASW in a 4-1 plastic beaker. The ASW had approximately the same osmolarity and temperature as the ASW in the aquarium from which the animals were removed, and had not previously contacted A. brasiliana. Animal-conditioned ASW not only increases the attractiveness of a non-laying cospecific, but also reduces the latency to mating (Painter et al., 1991).

Animals were rinsed in fresh non-conditioned ASW before being introduced into the experimental beaker.

Two individuals of A. brasiliana and a test sample were added to a beaker, and behaviors were assessed at 10-min intervals for 270 min. Three categories of behavior were identified: (1) mating as a female or male (one-way mating), (2) mating as a hermaphrodite, and (3) laying eggs. Since an egg cordon is a source of multiple contact and water-borne pheromones that modify reproductive behaviors, egg-laying activity was noted and the bioassay stopped; the bioassay for that sample was repeated with other animals. Egg laying occurred rarely with any stimulus.

Test samples included ASW with nothing added (negative control), ASW with 1 or 10 pmol native attractin added, and ASW with 1 pmol recombinant attractin added. The statistical significance of the differences between time points was determined by chi-square analysis; the statistical significance of differences in mean latency was determined by one-way analysis of variance. The same number of assays was performed for each treatment.

Results (native attractin). When 1 or 10 pmol of native attractin was placed in ASW containing two non-laying specimens of A. brasiliana, the percentage of animals mating at each time point (10-min intervals) was recorded. The percentage of animals mating was significantly increased for 10 pmol attractin at 10, 20, 30, and 40 min, and there was a nonsignificant trend in this direction for 1 pmol attractin (Fig. 4A). The mean latency to mating was significantly reduced for 10 pmol attractin ([chi square](1) > 3.84 for each; P < 0.05; n = 10), and there was a nonsignificant trend in this direction for 1 pmol (Fig. 4B). Although the latency to mating was reduced, the overall percentage of animal pairs mating during the 270-min period was not affected (negative controls: 90% mated; native attractin: 100% mated), perhaps reflecting the long duration of the assay or animal housing in individual cages. In these experiments, nearly all animal pairs eventually mated during the 270-min time period, regardless of whether attractin was present. Nevertheless, the results suggest that attractin facilitates, but does not induce, mating.

[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]

Results (recombinant attractin). When 1 pmol of recombinant attractin was placed in ASW containing two non-laying specimens of A. brasiliana, the percentage of animals mating at 10, 20, 170, and 240 min was significantly increased compared to negative controls [[chi square](1) > 3.84 for each; P < 0.05; n - 10; Fig. 4C]. The mean latency to mating was significantly reduced for 1 pmol recombinant attractin (P < 0.05; one-way analysis of variance; Fig. 4D). Although the latency to mating was reduced, the total percentage of animal pairs that mated during the entire 270-min period was sinrilar (negative controls: 90% mated; recombinant attractin: 100% mated), suggesting again that attractin facilitates, rather than induces, mating.

Pheromonal induction of hermaphroditic mating

Procedure. This is a re-analysis of the data collected in the mating assays, focusing on whether attractin can induce or facilitate hermaphroditic mating. As noted above, hermaphroditic mating was recorded during the mating assays.

Results (native attractin). When native attractin was placed in the ASW surrounding two non-laying specimens of A. brasiliana, the percentage of animal pairs mating as hermaphrodites was significantly increased for 10 pmol attractin at every time point between 20 and 170 min and for 190, 200, 230, and 250 min ([chi square](1) > 3.84 for each; P < 0.05; n = 10); the same was true for 1 pmol attractin at 230, 240, and 250 min ([chi square](1) > 3.84 for each; P < 0.05; n = 10) (Fig. 5A). The mean latency to hermaphroditic mating was significantly reduced for 10 pmol attractin (P < 0.05; one-way analysis of variance), and there was a nonsignificant trend in this direction for 1 pmol attractin (Fig. 5B). Compared to control assays, the percentage of animal pairs that mated as hermaphrodites during the 270-min period was about doubled (negative controls: 40%; 1 pmol: 70%; 10 pmol: 80%). This suggests that attractin induces, rather than facilitates, hermaphroditic mating, perhaps by stimulating both animals to mate as males. This induction could be responsible for copulatory rings and chains in the field, which may result because there are usually more than two animals in an aggregation.

Results (recombinant attractin). The percentage of animals mating as hermaphrodites at any given time point and the latency to hermaphroditic mating were not significantly increased upon addition of 1 pmol of recombinant attractin, although there were trends in this direction (Fig. 5 C, D). Although the percentage of animals mating as hermaphrodites was not significantly increased at any particular time point, the percentage of animal pairs that mated as hermaphrodites at some point during the assay did increase (negative controls: 40% mated as hermaphrodites; recombinant attractin: 70% mated as hermaphrodites). A dose of 10 pmol was not tested, which may account for the lack of statistical significance.

[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]

Discussion

We purified native attractin from extracts of Aplysia californica albumen gland (Painter et al., 1998) and recombinant attractin from insect cells to better characterize the biological activity of the peptide and to see whether recombinant attractin could be used in future molecular studies.

Pheromonal attraction

In the T-maze, the attractiveness of a stimulus animal was significantly increased when 1 pmol of either native or recombinant attractin was placed in the adjacent seawater, verifying that both peptides are attractive in amounts consistent with pherumonal activity, and confirming that N-glycosylation is not required for attraction. The response patterns for the two peptides do not differ significantly from each other, demonstrating that either could be used in future studies. Recombinant attractin was therefore used in subsequent T-maze bioassays. Since it was not N-glycosylated, recombinant attractin was also used to determine the solution structure of the pheromone by 3D nuclear magnetic resonance (Garimella et al., 2003).

Fewer individuals of A. brasiliana were attracted to recombinant attractin when the stimulus did not contain a non-laying conspecific, demonstrating that attractin acts in concert with other unidentified odors to stimulate attraction. These results, combined with earlier observations (the egg cordon is attractive without a stimulus animal, Painter et al., 1991; attractin elutes from the egg cordon, Painter et al., 1998), suggest that the composition of the bouquet of scents can vary. To identify other attractive chemical factors in the egg-cordon bouquet of scents, we have begun isolating other peptides/proteins that elute from the cordon for bioassay.

To begin looking for animal-derived attractants, we tested whether the stimulus animal needs to be a conspecific. It does not. A. californica with attractin and A. brasiliana with attractin each attracted a similar number of A. brasiliana. This pairing may seem inappropriate since the two species do not overlap in their geographic distributions (A. californica, Pacific Coast; A. brasiliana, Gulf of Mexico), but it may help explain why multiple Aplysia species are sometimes associated with one aggregation. For example, A. californica and A. vaccaria have been observed in the same breeding aggregations off the coast of California (Kupfermann and Carew, 1974; S. LePage, M-REP, pers. comm.), and have been seen mating with each other in the aggregation (S. LePage, M-REP, pers. comm.). A. fasciata and A. depilans have also been seen associated with the same aggregation (Achituv and Susswein, 1985), but mating has not been observed because their reproductive cycles are not entirely synchronized. Audesirk (1977) previously found that A. californica was not attracted to conspecifics in Y-maze assays, and Audesirk and Audesirk (1977) showed that there was no seasonal effect on the sensitivity to conspecifics. Furthermore, experimental perfusion of the A. californica rhinophore nerve with seawater that had bathed A. californica, A. vaccaria, or Pleurobranchia californica evoked about the same increase in afferent activity, suggesting that aggregations of Aplysia species in the field are not determined by species-specific chemical cues (Chase, 1979).

Pheromonal induction of mating

Mating assays were performed because behaviors seen in earlier T-maze assays suggested that exposure to attractin could stimulate behaviors suggestive of mating as a male (Painter et al., 1998). The current studies showed that when attractin is added to the seawater adjacent to a pair of A. brasiliana, the latency to mating is reduced relative to controls. However, the overall percentage of animal pairs mating during the prolonged assay period was not significantly different, suggesting that attractin facilitates, but does not induce, mating.

Attractin also significantly reduces the latency to hermaphroditic mating when added to the seawater surrounding a pair of A. brasiliana. The percentage of animal pairs mating as hermaphrodites during the assay period was about doubled, suggesting that attractin induces hermaphroditic mating. This effect may result from attractin stimulating both animals to mate as males, as suggested by T-maze behaviors. Overall, these data suggest that attractin contributes to the establishment and maintenance of breeding aggregations.

Attractin does not stimulate species-specific attraction

The attractins appear to be a structurally diverse family of peptides, each of which is sequence-specific for a given species. Attractin has recently been characterized from A. brasiliana, A. fasciata, A. vaccaria, A. depilans, and Bursatella leachii and found to be 95%, 91%, 43%, 41%, and 21% identical to A. californica attractim respectively (Painter et al., 2000, and unpubl, data). Nevertheless, attractin is attractive to all aquatic gastropods tested to date: (1) A. californica attractin is attractive to A. brasiliana (Painter et al., 1998); (2) A. vaccaria attractin is attractive to A. brasiliana (unpubl. data); and (3) A. californica attractin is attractive to the freshwater pulmonate Lymnaea stagnalis (A. ter Maat, Free University, Amsterdam, pers. comm.). Although the primary structures of attractin-related peptides are divergent, their 3D structures may be similar to A. californica attractin (Garimella et al., 2003). To our knowledge, the attractins are the first peptide pheromone family in invertebrates that is not species-specific. In contrast, waterborne peptide pheromonal attractants in amphibians are species-specific (Kikuyama et al., 2002).

There may be advantages to attracting multiple species to the same breeding aggregation. If members of a second species lay eggs on those of a different species, the mixed egg mass becomes larger, which might in some way protect the eggs of both species. Another possibility is that egg laying by one Aplysia species attracts a second species that then lays eggs and releases attractin, which may eventually attract members of the first species. Because attractin is continuously degraded from the C-terminus after its release (Painter et at., 1998, and unpubl, obs.), it may be advantageous to attract as many individual Aplysia as possible, regardless of species, to lay eggs and maintain the elevated attractin concentrations needed to recruit new individuals to the breeding aggregation.

Chemical communication frequently involves the use of blends of pheromones rather than single-compound pheromones. Blends of airborne pheromones are important for species-specific signaling in many organisms, including arthropods. Mate finding in most moth species, for example, involves the release of long-distance airborne sex pheromones, which are produced in specialized female abdominal glands, generally via unsaturated fatty-acid precursors produced by desaturases (Roelofs et al., 2002). A great diversity of pheromone structures is used throughout the Lepidoptera, even among closely related species, and the blend ratio is important for species-specific signaling. There is strong selection pressure against novel blends and response preferences (Roelofs et al., 2002). Although airborne sex pheromones capable of inducing spatial orientation of conspecifics "downwind" are well established in insects (Carde and Minks, 1996), this is not the case in vertebrates, whose identified sex pheromones tend to have a small range of effectiveness; in fish, the known sex pheromones are gonadal steroids, prostaglandins, or bile acids (Li et al., 2002).

Mate attraction in the genus Aplysia, and perhaps in other gastropods, appears to involve long-distance signaling via waterborne pheromone blends. Attraetin by itself is not attractive to Aplysia species, but egg cordons alone are suflicient to attract Aplysia species "downstream," indicating that the cordons themselves contain a blend of pheromones. Once aggregations of multiple Aplysia species form, appropriate intraspecific mating may be achieved through the use of specific proximal cues involving contact chemoreception and mechanoreception (Chase, 1979).

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. A. Susswein, A. ter Maat, and M. Miller for their interesting comments and acknowledge the University of Texas Medical Branch (UTMB) Protein Chemistry Laboratory, which is supported by the UTMB Educational Cancer Center, lor compositional and microsequence analyses. This work was supported by NSF Grant IBN-9985778 (S.D.P.), and John Sealy Memorial Endowment Fund for Biomedical Research Development Grant 2579-02R (G.T.N.).

Literature Cited

Achituv, Y., and A. Susswein. 1985. Habitat selection by two Mediterranean species of Aplysia: A. fasciata Poiret and A. depilans Gmelin (Mollusca: Opisthobranchia). J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 85: 113-122.

Aspey, W. P., and J. E. Blankenship. 1976. Aph,sia behavioral biology. II. Induced burrowing in swimming A. brasiliana by burrowed conspecifics. Behav. Biol. 17: 301-312.

Audesirk, T. E. 1977. Chemoreception in Aplysia californica. III. Evidence for pheromones influencing reproductive behavior. Behav. Biol. 20: 235-243.

Audesirk, T. E. 1979. A field study of growth and reproduction in Aplysia californica. Biol. Bull. 157: 407-421.

Audesirk, T. E., and G. J. Audesirk. 1977. Chemoreception in Aplysia californica. II. Electrophysiological evidence for detection of the odor of conspeciflcs. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 56A: 267-270.

Blankenship, J. E., M. K. Rock, L. C. Robhins, C. A. Livingston, and H. K. Lehman. 1983. Aspects of copulatory behavior and peptide control of egg laying in Aplysia. Fed. Proc. 42: 96-100.

Carde, R. T., and A. K. Minks. 1996. Molecular mechanisms of pheromone reception in insect antennae. Pp. 115-130 in Insect Pheromone Research, H. Breer, ed. Chapman and Hall, New York.

Chase, R. 1979. An electrophysiological search for pheromones of Aplysia californica. Can. J. Zool. 57: 781-784.

Fan, X., B. Wu, G. T. Nagle, and S. D. Painter. 1997. Molecular cloning of a cDNA encoding a potential water-borne pheromonal attractant released during Aplysia egg laying. Mol. Brain Res. 48: 167-170.

Garimella, R., Y. Xu, C. H. Schein, K. Rajarathnam, G. T. Nagle, S. D. Painter, and W. Braun. 2003. NMR solution structure of attractin, a water-borne protein pheromone from the mollusk Aplysia californica. Biochemistry. (In press).

Jahan-Parwar, B. 1976. Aggregation pheromone from file egg-mass of Aplysia californica. Physiologist 19: 240.

Kikuyama, S., F. Toynda, Y. Ohmiya, K. Matsuda, S. Tanaka, and H. Hayashi. 1995. Sodefrin: a female-attracting peptide pheromone in newt cloacal glands. Science 267: 1643-1645.

Kikuyama, S., K. Yamamoto, T. Iwata, and F. Toyoda. 2002. Peptide and protein pheromones in amphibians. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B 132: 69-74.

Kodama, T., T. Hisatomi, T. Kanemura, K. Mokubo, and M. Tsuboi. 2003. Molecular cloning and DNA analysis of a gene encoding alpha mating pheromone from the yeast Saccharomyees naganishii. Yeast 20: 109-115.

Kupfermann, I., and T. Carew. 1974. Behavior patterns of Aplysia californica in its natural environment. Behav. Biol. 12: 317-337.

Li, W., A. P. Scott, M. J. Siefkes, H. Yah, Q. Liu, S.-S. Yun, and D. A. Gage. 2002. Bile acid secreted by male sea lamprey that acts as a sex pheromone. Science 296: 138-140.

Luporini, P., A. Vallnsi, C. Miceli, and It. A. Bradshaw. 1995. Chemical signaling in ciliates. J. Eukaryot. Microbiol. 42: 208-212.

Monsma, S. A., and M. F. Wolfner. 1988. Structure and expression of a Drosophila male accessory gland gene whose product resembles a peptide pheromone precursor. Genes Dee. 2: 10631073.

Novotny, M. V. 2003. Pheromones, binding proteins and receptor responses in rodents. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 31:117-122.

Painter, S. D. 1992. Coordination of reproductive activity in Aplysia: peptide neurohormoues, neurotransmitters, and pheromones encoded by the egg-laying hormone family of genes. Biol, Bull. 183: 165-172.

Painter, S. D., A. R. Gustavson, V. K. Kalman, G. T. Nagle, R. A. Zuckerman, and J. E. Blankenship. 1989. Induction of copulatory behavior in Aplysia: atrial gland factors mimic the excitatory effects of recently deposited egg cordons. Behav. Neural Biol. 51: 222-236.

Painter, S. D., M. G. Chong, M. A. Wong, A. Gray, J. G. Cormier, and G. T. Nagle. 1991. Relative contributions of the egg layer and egg cordon to phernmonal attraction and the induction of mating and egg-laying behavior in Aplysia. Biol. Bull. 181: 81-94.

Painter, S. D., B. Clough, R. W. Garden, J. V. Sweedler, and G. T. Nagle. 1998. Characterization of Aplysia attractin, the first waterborne peptide pheromone in invertebrates. Biol, Bull, 194: 120-131.

Painter, S. D., D.-B. G. Akalal, B. Clough, A. J. Susswein, M. Levy, and G. T. Nagle. 2000. Characterization of four new members of the attractin family of peptide pheromones in Aplysia. Soc. Neurosei. Abstr. 26: 1166.

Pennings, S. C. 1991. Reproductive behavior of Aplysia californica Cooper: diel patterns, sexual roles and mating aggregations. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 149: 249-266.

Ram, J. L., C. T. Muller, M. Beckmann, and J. D. Hardege. 1999. The spawning pheromone cysteine-glutathione disulfide ('nereithione') arouses a multicomponent nuptial behavior and electrophysiological activity in Nereis succinea males. FASEB J. 13: 945-952.

Roelofs, W. L., W. Liu, G. Hao, H. Jiao, A. P. Rooney, and C. E. Linn, Jr. 2002. Evolution of moth sex pheromones via ancestral genes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99:13621 13626.

Rollmann, S. M., L D. Houck, and R. C. Feldhoff. 1999. Protein aceous pheromone affecting female receptivity in a terrestrial salamander. Science 285: 1907-1909.

Saudan, P., K. Hauck, M. Soller, Y. Choffat, M. Ottiger, M. Sporri, Z. Ding, D. Hess, P. M. Gehrig, S. Klauser, P. Hunziker, and E. Kubli. 2002. Ductus ejaculatorius peptide 99B (DUP99B), a novel Drosophila melanogaster sex-peptide pheromone. Eur. J. Biochem. 269: 989-997.

Savic, I., H. Berglund, B. Gulyas, and P. Roland. 2001. Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans. Neuron 31: 661-668.

Schein, C. H., G. T. Nagle, J. S. Page, J. V. Sweedler, Y. Xu, S. D. Painter, and W. Braun. 2001. Aplysia attractin: biophysical characterization and modeling of a water-borne pheromone. Biophysical J. 81: 463-472.

Stowers, L., T. E. Holy, M. Meister, C. Dulac, and G. Koentges. 2002. Loss of sex discrimination and male-male aggression in mice deficient for TRP2. Science 295: 1493-15130.

Susswein, A. J., S. Gee, E. Feldman, and S. Markovitch. 1983. Activity patterns and time budgeting of Aplysia fasciata in field and laboratory conditions. Behav. Neural BioL 39: 203-220.

Susswein, A. J., S. Gee, Y. Achituv, and S. Markovitch. 1984. Behav ioral patterns of Aplysia fasciata along the Mediterranean coast of Israel. Behav. Neural Biol, 41: 203-220.

Wabnitz, P. A., J. H. Bowie, M. J. Tyler, J. C. Wallace, and B. P. Smith. 1999. Aquatic sex pheromone from a male tree frog. Nature 401:444 445.

SHERRY D. PAINTER *, BRET CLOUGH, SARA BLACK, AND GREGG T. NAGLE

The Marine Biomedical Institute and the Department of Anatomy and Neurosciences, University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston, Texas 77555-1069

Received 8 October 2002; accepted 16 April 2003.

* To whom correspondence should be addressed. Marine Biomedical Institute, 2.138 Medical Research Building, University of Texas Medical Branch, 301 University Blvd., Galveston, Texas 77555-1069. E-mail: sdpainter@houston.rr.com

Abbreviations: ASW, artificial seawater; Att, attractin; C[H.sub.3]CN, acetonitrile; HFBA, heptafluorobutyric acid; M-REP, Marine Research and Educational Products: RP-HPLC, reversed-phase high performance liquid chromatography.
COPYRIGHT 2003 University of Chicago Press
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2003 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:Neurobiology and Behavior
Author:Painter, Sherry D.; Clough, Bret; Black Sara; Nagle, Gregg T.
Publication:The Biological Bulletin
Date:Aug 1, 2003
Words:6140
Previous Article:250 million years of bindin evolution.
Next Article:Field observations of intraspecific agonistic behavior of two Crayfish species, Orconectes rusticus and Orconectes virilis, in different habitats.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2019 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters