Printer Friendly

Alloying as an economy principle in morphology.

1. Introduction

A misconception in a number of influential modern linguistic theories is that there is exactly one syntactic argument per semantic referent in a clause, as Croft (2002: 228) argues with reference to the Theta Criterion in Generative Grammar and the Uniqueness Condition in Lexical-Functional Grammar. Croft (2002: 226-233) presents various phenomena that are problematic for such one-to-one mapping, as in the following sentence from the Australian language Warlpiri.

(1) wawirr-o kapi-rna-o panti-rni yalumpu-o

kangaroo-ABS FUT-1SG.SU spear-NPST that-ABS

'I will spear that kangaroo'

Croft (202: 227) points out that there is no independent pronoun in this example, so "[h]ow can there be a syntactic relation between two elements if the other element (independent pronoun in this case) is not there?" He concludes that the person-marking affix expresses a symbolic relation, indexing the referent rather than the phrase denoting the referent.

This paper presents examples from various African languages in order to argue that similar symbolic relations may occur at the word level. More specifically, one widespread verbal derivational strategy, that of Ventive marking (attested in at least three African language phyla), is discussed in order to show that--contrary to what is sometimes claimed--a single verb may in fact designate two causally unrelated sub-events, here involving manner and change of location. Whereas the first event is expressed by means of a lexical root, the second is expressed by means of a derivational suffix, as I will show for the Afroasiatic Chadic language Hausa (Section 2.1), the Niger-Congo language Tima (Section 2.2), and a number of Nilo-Saharan Nilotic languages (Section 2.3).

A further aim of this study is to provide an explanation for this widespread morphosyntactic and lexical phenomenon. I will argue that the explanation is probably to be found in economy principles operating in languages. The symbolic conflation of different roles into one grammatical word, or " alloying", provides advantages in terms of language production and processing for the hearer (Section 3).

2. Conflating the mapping of semantic roles

Talmy's (1985, 2000) cross-linguistic typology of lexicalisation patterns makes a distinction between satellite-framed and verb-framed languages. In the former, manner of motion, for example, tends to be expressed on the verb, whereas path of motion is expressed separately, for example by particles. This pattern is common in Germanic languages, whereas Romance languages tend to be verb-framed.

Slobin's (2004: 249) motion-event language typology lists different semantic and morphosyntactic properties of these three language types (Table 1).

Ameka and Essegbey (2013) have argued that serial verb constructions in Kwa (Niger-Congo) languages like Ewe indeed represent an additional type that Slobin (2004, 2010) refers to as "equipollent". Languages of this type do not subordinate manner expressions as Vlanguage do" (Ameka and Essegbey 2013: 36). The following example of Ewe serial verbs illustrate this third strategy.

(2) kpe'-a' mli ye do-a me

stone-DEF roll go hole-DEF containing region

'the stone rolled into the hole'

Verb framing may involve preverbs, serial verbs or verbal compounding (Slobin 2010). But evidence from various African languages suggests that verb-framed languages may employ an additional strategy. A widespread verbal extension, that of "movement towards the deictic centre", illustrates the relevance of this additional strategy. This derivational marker is commonly referred to as the Ventive in descriptions of African languages.

In an early source Tucker and Mpaayei (1955: 123) show that in Maasai, an Eastern Nilotic Nilo-Saharan language spoken in Kenya and Tanzania, this marker expresses action towards the speaker or the main person in the sentence (interlinear glossing added by the present author).

(3) ta-lak-u nkishu

IMP-release-VEN cattle

untie the cattle (and bring them) here

Tucker and Mpaayei (1955: 126-129) also show that in Maasai the Ventive marker is in paradigmatic contrast with a marker usually referred to as Itive or Andative in modern studies of derivational strategies in African languages.

(4) a-rew-aa nkishu

ISG-drive-IT cattle

'I shall drive the cattle right away'

For reasons of space, I will concentrate on Ventive marking to illustrate the phenomenon of alloying, although it also exists elsewhere, with Itive marking for example. As is common with derivational processes in general, the semantic mapping of such derived (Ventive or Itive) verbs may also involve more or less unpredictable semantic elaborations through metaphorical extensions into more abstract meanings, as in the following example from Tucker and Mpaayei (1955: 125), who point out that 'eat in this direction' stands for 'tell'.

(5) inos-u lomon

eat-VEN news

'tell the news!'

This paper focuses on the concrete, spatial reference implied by the presence of the Ventive marker, since metaphorical extension as such is not pertinent to the issue.

In their pioneering study, Tucker and Mpaayei (1955: 124) give examples of alloying.

(6) e-te-dek-ua yiook

3 -NARR-curse-VEN 1PL

'1. (s)he came cursing us;

2. (s)he cursed as we came'

As the following brief survey illustrates, similar alloying strategies are attested in three unrelated and geographically non-contiguous African language families, which suggests that there is a deeper cognitive principle hidden behind this phenomenon.

2.1 Hausa (Afroasiatic)

Ventive markers are among the most widespread verbal extensions in the Chadic branch of Afroasiatic (Frajzyngier 2012: 264). Newman (2000: 661-663) gives a detailed description of this derivational marker for one Chadic language, Hausa, a major lingua franca spoken primarily in Nigeria and Niger. The Ventive is part of a system involving three basic grades (consisting of non-derived verbs belonging to grades 1, 2 or 3), and four derived grades (4 Totality/Finality; 5 Efferential; 6 Ventive; 7 Sustentative). Grade 6 verbs all end in a long vowel -oo (commonly written with a macron, -o, in Hausa reference grammars or pedagogical grammars), whereby the verb carries high tone throughout. "The ventive ending generally denotes action or movement in the direction of the speaker (or any other pragmatically established deictic center), sometimes emphasizing the distance of the occurrence from the speaker" (Newman 2000: 663). This is illustrated in example 7 (interlinear glossing by the present author).

(7) kogi-n Binuwai ya far-o daga

river-GEN Benue 3SG:M begin-VEN from

fasa-r Kamaru

land-GEN Cameroon

The River Benue begins (there) in Cameroon (and then comes this way) (2)

But as Newman (2000: 663) points out, "gr[ade] 6 sometimes indicates 'do some action and come'", as in the following examples.

(8) na shaf-o bango

1SG whitewash-VEN wall

'I whitewashed the wall and came back'

(9) ya say-o nama

3SG:M buy-VEN meat

'he bought some meat and brought it back here'

It is this latter type of construction, involving two sub-events whereby an Agent carries out one action followed by another action without the latter being expressed by a separate second verb, which is central to the present study. Instead of using a separate verb 'come' (2) or 'bring (3) to express motion or path for a Figure (as one would in a serializing language), a derivational extension is added to the verb to express one "macro-event" (Talmy 1991, Bohnemeyer et al. 2007), namely one sub-event expressing some action carried out by the subject followed by an event denoting location change (movement towards the deictic center) involving the same subject, with the latter event being expressed by the Ventive marker.

It should be emphasized that the Ventive marker is a grammatical rather than a lexical root in Hausa (as it is in the other languages discussed below), i.e. this is not a case of verbal compounding with two lexical roots. The complex event expressed by this verb would count as a macro-event in the typology of Bohnemeyer et al. (2007) since negation marking (expressed by double negation in Hausa) or adverbial modification would affect both events, these being expressed in one and the same word.

(10) ba?y say-o nama ba

NEG.3SG:M buy-VEN meat NEG

'he did not buy and bring back (some) meat here'

2.2 Tima (Niger-Congo)

Ventive marking on verbs is also attested in Niger-Congo, for example in languages of the Nuba Mountains in Sudan. Alamin, Schneider-Blum and Dimmendaal (2012) give a description of the Ventive marker (and of directionality and location marking in general) in Tima.3 Source, location, and goal are marked periphrastically by way of proclitic prepositional elements. In this respect, Tima may be classified as a satellite-framed language. But there is one verbal suffix expressing motion or path towards the deictic center (usually the speaker), as example (11) illustrates. (4)

(11) KwAkwAn an-duwa-y-in a-li-?ee

KwAkwAn PER-go.down-EP-VEN PREP-LOC-east

'Kwakwan went down/came from the east (towards the speaker)'

Tima appears to be unique among African languages with Ventive marking in that the position of the speaker (EGO) is also expressed in the prepositional phrase, if there is one, and expresses information on the position of the speaker:

(12) diI-y-AAn nAteA-lah

Walk-EP-VEN LOC:EGO-field

'come to the field (where I am)'

The deictic marker on the prepositional phrase expressing the position of EGO may also occur without a corresponding Ventive marker on the verb.

(13) wer temaadah an-tuw-ak-ag-aa curan ntu-tende

man PER-drop-AP-VEN-INST stick LOC:EGO-street

the man threw the stick into the street (where the speaker was)

Dimmendaal (2013) interprets Ventive marking on verbs and marking the position of the speaker on prepositional phrases in Tima as an evidentiality marking strategy, as these markers express the fact that the speaker is or was a witness of the event. Alternatively, by omitting these morphological strategies, the conversational implicature is that the speaker is, was or will not be a witness of the event expressed by the verb.

Tima, like Hausa, may combine the Ventive marker with lexical roots in order to express a second, subsecutive event, as in the imperative verb forms in examples 14 and 15. If the core meaning is a non-motion event, the conversational implicature is that the act expressed by the verbal root is performed first before the Agent (as the Figure) moves in the direction of the speaker.

(14) moak-ip

drink-VEN

'drink and come (to where I am)!'

(15) meek-in

build-VEN house

'build the house and come (to where I am)'

Constituent order in clauses such as (15) may be considered non-iconic in that the object complement ('house') and the verbal event associated with it ('build') are discontinuous, due to the intervening Ventive marker which expresses a subsequent event.

Morphologically complex verbs in (14) and (15) express macro-events (Bohnemeyer et al. 2007), in that negation markers or time adverbs have scope over both events expressed by one phonological and grammatical word (no tones indicated).

(16) kaa-mook-in=an

NEG.2SG-drink-VEN-NEG

'do not drink and (do not) come (to where I am)!'

In other words, in Tima it is not only multi-verb constructions that may have macro-event properties; single verbs may also.

2.3 Nilotic (Nilo-Saharan)

Ventive marking is particularly prominent in a third African phylum, Nilo-Saharan. Whereas the formal expression differs between languages (i.e. not all Ventive markers are cognate), the concept itself is a stable property of the derivational morphology of verbs in this phylum (Dimmendaal 2014: 600). One important reason for this may be the fact that this morpheme again plays an important role as an evidentiality marker (stating that the speaker witnessed or witnesses an event), as in Tima. In this discussion of Nilo-Saharan, I will focus on Ventive marking in one major subgroup, Nilotic, because it illustrates two additional structural properties of this marker and of alloying in general, namely that the pivot may be expressed not only as a subject (prototypically the Agent) but also as an object (usually expressing the Patient or Theme). Moreover, Nilotic languages show that the second sub-event, marked by way of the Ventive marker, may also express a simultaneous (rather than a subsequent) event.

In order to understand this type of construction in Nilotic, a brief outline of argument structures and corresponding formal marking is in order. Tucker and Mpaayei (1955: 187) show that Maasai postverbal subjects are inflected for (Nominative) case by means of tone. Moreover, when a first or second person is the object, cross-reference marking for the latter occurs by means of prefixes on the verb.

(17) aa-del elmeut monruo

3>1-see giraffe:NOM old

the old giraffe sees me

Nouns, noun phrases, proper names or independent pronouns take Absolutive case in Maasai unless they occur as postverbal subjects (in which case they take Nominative case, as in 17) above. Tucker and Mpaayei (1955) call this unmarked case form Accusative rather than Absolutive. It should be noted, however, that the "Accusative" case form is also used for preverbal (as opposed to postverbal) subjects, predicative nouns, nouns in isolation etc. Consequently, I prefer the term Absolutive. Examples 18 and 19 illustrate the tonal alternation for the proper name Sironka in subject and object position (Tucker and Mpaayei 1955: 175176).

(18) a-dol Sironka

1SG-see Sironka:ABS

I see Sironka'

(19) aa-dol Sironka

3>1-see Sironka:NOM

Sironka sees me'

The passive in Maasai is expressed by means of a suffix (whose actual form depends on the different Tense-Aspect-Mood categories with which it occurs). As is typical in Nilotic, objects remain objects (i.e. no object-to-subject raising occurs) and retain their Absolutive case marking.

(20) e-isis-i Sironka

3-praise-PASS Sironka:ABS

'Sironka is praised; one praises Sironka'

Also, pronominal objects remain in object position in passives, and the same cross-reference marking occurs as with active constructions, as a comparison with (19) above shows (no tone marking given).

(21) aa-re-un-i

3>1 -drive-VEN-PAS S

'I am driven this way'

In Maasai and other Nilotic languages, pronominal subject and object marking by way of independent pronouns in addition to the verbal affixes only occurs for pragmatic reasons, e.g. in order to focus upon or topicalize such a constituent. Thus, in example (21) above, the Absolutive pronoun nanu 'me' may be added for pragmatic reasons, but syntactically it is superfluous.

In example 22 (Tucker and Mpaayei 1955: 124), such a direct object constitutes the pivot of an alloying construction involving two events: An (unexpressed) Agent (expressed as subject) seeing or observing somebody, and a Patient (expressed as object) moving in the direction of the Agent (no tones indicated). (5)

(22) e-dol-un-i

3-see-VEN-PASS

'(s)he is seen coming this way'

Whereas in Hausa and Tima, the Ventive expresses a subsecutive event, the Maasai Ventive marker usually (though not always) expresses a simultaneous event that again is causally unrelated to the event expressed by the verb root. In fact, this is a general characteristic of Eastern Nilotic. How could this difference have come about? A closer look at related languages of the Southern Nilotic branch provides a possible historical explanation. (6)

Contrary to Eastern Nilotic languages like Maasai, Southern Nilotic languages make a formal distinction between direction involving movement or not. Mietzner (2009) gives a detailed description of these phenomena and of spatial orientation in general in Nilotic languages. She, along with other authors (e.g Rottland 1982, KieBling 2007), shows that a separate Mobilitive marker is added to the Ventive marker (expressing direction) in order to show that movement is involved. Rottland (1982: 244-245) reconstructs the Ventive marker as *-u for Proto-Southern Nilotic. Synchronically, this marker is often realized through vowel shift on a preceding verb root. The same author reconstructs the Mobilitive marker as *-aanwhen preceding the Ventive marker (hence Mobilitive-Ventive: *-aan-u) for Proto-Southern Nilotic. Synchronically, the suffixation of these and other verb derivation markers usually involves complex morphophonological alternations, but these should not concern us here. Examples 23 and 24 are from Datooga (adapted from Rottland 1982: 186).
(23)  nilag-u         (s)he has cut (my way)'
      3:cut:VEN

(24)  nilaj-aani      1. '(s)he has been cutting while coming my way'
      3.cut:MOB.VEN   2. '(s)he came while cutting'


Kiessling (2007: 130) refers to the additional morpheme in example (24) as a Mobilitive marker, and also gives examples of alloying constructions whereby the root event refers to the subject (Agent) and the event expressed by the Ventive marker refers to the object (Patient) moving towards the speaker (or the deictic center), parallel to the Maasai example in (22) above.

(25) qwa-daahaan duga [...]

3SG-see.MOB.VEN cattle

'he saw the cattle coming his way [...]'

There is no corresponding Mobilitive marker with the same function in Eastern Nilotic languages. In this Nilotic branch, the (presumably) cognate marker--aan--functions as a Habitual or Inchoative marker. In Maasai, the Ventive may express both simultaneous and subsecutive events, depending on the verb root to which it is attached (examples 3 and 6). Possibly, then, the Ventive marker in Eastern Nilotic languages expresses not only subsecutive but also simultaneous events because the Mobilitive marker is no longer available for this semantic role. As in Hausa and Tima, it is more common for the Ventive marker in Eastern Nilotic languages to express a subsecutive, rather than simultaneous events. Examples 26 and 27, from the Eastern Nilotic language Teso (Otaala 1981: 98, 103), illustrate this. (Interlinear glossing provided by the present author).

(26) a-ikis-un

INF-scratch off.VEN

'scratch off something and bring here'

(27) e-nyounit-os apesur kere a-ipo-un inyamat ne

3-intend-PL girls all INF-cook-VEN food here

'all girls intend to cook and bring the food here'

Because of the strong tendency towards fusion and internal morphology in Western Nilotic languages, it is not clear whether the actual forms are cognate with the Eastern and Southern Nilotic markers. Nevertheless, clear cases of alloying expressing subsecutive rather than simultaneous events can be found, for example in Mabaan (Andersen 1999: 109).

(28) Jekken diiep ween-n-e

3PL cow tie-VEN-PAST.3PL3

'they brought the cow and tied it here'

These Nilotic examples raise more general questions. What combinations of sub-events can a single verb refer to, and are these to be interpreted as simultaneous or subsecutive events? The answer appears to be that verb meanings are possible as long as they describe events that form a coherent semantic and culturally meaningful frame for speakers and hearers in a given speech community. (7) Alloying in these languages shows that a single verb can designate two causally unrelated sub-events which do not necessarily overlap temporally either, although they may do so (namely when expressing simultaneous events). Moreover, the second event is not caused by the first. Instead, it constitutes a frame for another event. This does not necessarily lead towards unwarranted ambiguity, as knowledge of the real world helps to make sense of the intended meaning.

3. Language production and language processing

Whenever a speaker was or is capable of observing some action or event happening, the information is presumably more reliable than indirect information deriving from hearsay. Speakers apparently prefer to be explicit about this cognitive state, as suggested by the widespread use of Ventive markers expressing or specifying the location of the speaker. The inferred meaning or conversational implicature may therefore be assigned to the field of evidentiality marking.

Interestingly, the Ventive marker expressing this cognitive experience may also be attached to verb roots. Alloying, or the conflation of two morphemes expressing separate verbal events into one phonological and grammatical word in three distinct languages which are geographically wide apart and belong to three unrelated phyla strongly suggest that there is a common cognitive basis for this phenomenon.

The most plausible explanation for this type of verb framing may be found in a wellknown linguistic phenomenon, the economy principle. (8) This principle may apparently override diagrammatic iconicity in that no one-to-one mapping of elements into components of the semantic structure is necessarily required.

As example (22) above shows, the semantic relations between those components are complex (involving Agent as well as Patient relations) and often highly context-sensitive. From a speaker's point of view, this conflation of sememes expressing different events into one phonological word results in a clear property enhancement, namely the conveyance of a complex situation or event structure in an economic way. Slobin (2004) points out that expression by a single word rather than a phrase or clause as a shortcut strategy also provides advantages for the hearer in terms of language processing.

Alloying allows speakers to designate a co-occurring activity which is not directly or causally related to the action or event expressed by the verb root, but which nevertheless is relevant to the event described by the verb root. In the concrete case of the Ventive, this dimension is evidentiality, i.e. stating that one witnesses or witnessed the event. Such collocations usually do not refer to established semantic frames (as the Ventive can occur fairly freely with many verbs). But these collocations may lexicalize and become a single culturally recognized unit stored as a template in the long-term memory, as the following lexicalized forms illustrate. In Hausa, such lexicalized verbs may express direction or movement and at the same time the manner in which the event occurs (Newman 1977: 17).

(29) 6ull-o 'appear suddenly or unexpectedly'

When the Path component is tightly packaged in a language, this potentially results in the expression of Manner and Path in one and the same verb.

Similarly, idiomatic examples in Maasai show that specific semantic frames may become conventionalized (Tucker and Mpaayei 1955: 125).

(30) a-inas-u 'tell (lit. eat in this direction)'

INF-eat-VEN

Presumably, what is necessary is that the situation or experience evokes a cultural unit that is familiar and meaningful to those who use the word.
Abbreviations

ABS  = absolutive
AP   = antipassive
DEF  = definiteness
EP   = epenthetic
F    = feminine
FUT  = future
GEN  = genitive
IMP  = imperative
INF  = infinitive
INST = instrumental
IT   = itive
LOV  = locative
M    = masculine
MOB  = mobilitive
NARR = narrative
NEG  = negative
NOM  = nominative
NPST = non-past
PASS = passive
PER  = perfect
PL   = plural
PREP = preposition
SG   = singular
SU   = subject
VEN  = ventive


References

Alamin, Suzan & Schneider-Blum, Gertrud & Dimmendaal, Gerrit J., 2012. Finding your way in Tima. In Angelika Mietzner and Ulrike Claudi (eds.), Directionality in grammar and discourse: Case studies from Africa. Cologne: Rudiger Koppe. 9-33.

Ameka, Felix K. & Essegbey, James. 2013. Serialising languages: satellite-framed, verb-framed or neither. Ghana Journal of Linguistics 2(1). 19-38.

Andersen, Torben. 1988. Consonant alternation in the verbal morphology of Pari. Afrika und Ubersee 71. 63-113.

Andersen, Torben. 1999. Vowel quality alternation in Mabaan and its Western Nilotic history. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 20(2). 97-120.

Bohnemeyer, Jurgen & Enfield, Nicholas J. & Essegbey, James & Ibarretxe-Antun(r)ano, Iraide & Kita, Sotaro & Lupke, Friederike & Ameka, Felix K. 2007. Principles of event segmentation in language: The case of motion events. Language 83(3). 495-532.

Bohnemeyer, Jurgen & Pederson, Eric. (eds.). 2010. Event representation in language and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Croft, William. 1991. Syntactic categories and grammatical relations. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Croft, William. 2002. Radical construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2000. Morphology. In Bernd Heine, and Derek Nurse (eds.), African languages: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 161-193.

Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2010. The grammar of knowledge in Tima. In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon (eds.), The grammar of knowledge. Leiden: Brill. 245-259.

Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2014. Derivation in Nilo-Saharan. In, Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Stekauer (eds.), Oxford handbook of derivation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 591-608.

Fillmore, Charles J. 1977. Topics in lexical semantic. In Roger W. Cole (ed.), Current issues in linguistic theory. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. 76-138.

Frajzyngier, Zygmunt & Shay, Erin. 2012. Chadic. In Zygmunt Frajzyngier and Erin Shay (eds.), The Afroasiatic languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 236-341.

Greenberg, Joseph H. 1963. The languages of Africa. Bloomington/The Hague: Indiana University Press.

KieBling, Roland. 2007. Space and reference in Datooga verbal morphosyntax. In Doris Payne and Mechthild Reh (eds.), Advances in Nilo-Saharan linguistics. Cologne: Rudiger Koppe. 123142.

Lord, Carol. 1975. Igbo verb compounds and the lexicon. In Studies in African Linguistics 6. 23-48.

Mietzner, Angelika. 2009. Rdumliche Orientierung in nilotischen Sprachen: Raumkonzepte Direktionalitut -Perspektiven. Cologne: Rudiger Koppe.

Newman, Paul. 2000. The Hausa language: An encyclopedic reference grammar. New Haven and London: Yale University Press.

Otaala, Laura A. 1981. Phonological and semantic aspects of Ateso derivational verbal morphology. University of Nairobi. (MA thesis)

Rottland, Franz. 1982. Die sudnilotischen Sprachen. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.

Slobin, D. I. 2004. The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events. In Sven Stromqvist and Ludo Verhoeven (eds.), Relating Events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 219257.

Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description Vol. 3: Grammatical categories and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 57-149.

Talmy, Leonard. 1991. Path to realization: A typology of event conflation. In Proceedings of the seventeenth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. 480-519.

Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics Vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tucker, Archibald Norman & Mpaayei, John Tompo Ole. 1955. A Maasai grammar with vocabulary. London: Longmans, Green and Co.

Gerrit J. Dimmendaal

Institute for African Studies and Egyptology

University of Cologne

Albertus-Magnus-Platz

D-50923 Koln-Lindenthal

gerrit.dimmendaal@uni-koeln.de

(1) I would like to express my special thanks to Dave Roberts for his critical comments on an earlier draft of the present contribution.

(2) I have added morpheme boundaries to facilitate the interpretation of these forms. It should be noted, however, that Hausa is a fusional language that does not usually allow for the identification of morpheme boundaries.

(3) According to Greenberg (1963), Tima belongs to the Kordofanian branch of Niger-Congo, but Dimmendaal (2011: 252) questions the genetic unity of this proposed grouping. This does not concern us here, since Tima' s affiliation to Niger-Congo is beyond doubt.

(4) Tima data were collected between 2006 and 2012 as part of an endangered language documentation project financed by the Volkswagen Foundation. I gratefully acknowledge this support.

(5) Unfortunately, no examples are available with alloying and a first or second person object.

(6) Nilotic is usually divided into three primary branches: Eastern Nilotic, Southern Nilotic, and Western Nilotic; the former two branches are probably more closely related to each other than they are to Western Nilotic.

(7) The term "frame" goes back to Fillmore (1977); it expresses an abstraction of an indivisible perception, memory, experience, action or object.

(8) Dimmendaal (2000: 185-187) gives further examples of non-iconic phenomena in morphology.
Table 1 Lexicalisation patterns (Slobin 2004)

Language type      Preferred means   Typical           Examples
                   of expression     construction
                                     type

Verb-framed        Path is           Path verb +       Romance,
languages          expressed by a    subordinate       Semitic,
                   verb, with        Manner verb       Turkic, Basque,
                   subordinate                         Japanese,
                   expression of                       Korean
                   manner

Satellite-framed   Path is           Manner verb +     Germanic,
languages          expressed by a    Path satellite    Slavic,
                   non-verbal                          Finno-Ugric
                   element
                   associated with
                   the verb

Equipollently-     Path and manner   Manner verb +     Niger-Congo,
framed languages   are expressed     Path verb         Hmong-Mien,
                   by equivalent     (serial-verb      Sino-Tibetan,
                   grammatical       languages)        Tai-Kadai,
                   forms                               Mon-Khmer,
                                     [Manner + Path]   Austronesian
                                     verb              Algonquian,
                                     (bipartite-       Athabaskan,
                                     verb languages)   Hokan,
                                                       Klamath-T
                                                       akelman

                                     Manner preverb    Jaminjungan
                                     + Path preverb    languages
                                     + verb
COPYRIGHT 2015 The Slovak Association for the Study of English
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2015 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

 
Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Dimmendaal, Gerrit J.
Publication:SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:60AFR
Date:Dec 1, 2015
Words:4423
Previous Article:Interview with Klaus Schneider.
Next Article:Analyzing the COG and intensity of Kermani vowels in the process of vowel reduction.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2018 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters