Printer Friendly

Adaptive radiation along genetic lines of least resistance.

Recent years have seen an increasingly wider appreciation that patterns of genetic (including developmental) variation constrain and direct the pathways of evolution (Bonner 1982; Maynard Smith et al. 1985; Futuyma 1988; Wake 1991). The promise of quantitative genetic theory was that it would enable precise predictions of such effects in natural populations for polygenic traits under natural selection (Lande 1979; Barton and Turelli 1989; Arnold 1992). This promise is being realized for evolutionary change across a single generation (Grant and Grant 1993, 1995).

The value of quantitative genetic theory for predicting long-term evolution is more doubtful. This is primarily because levels of genetic variance and covariance in evolving populations may not be sufficiently stable over time to permit prediction beyond a few generations (Lofsvold 1986; Turelli 1988; Barton and Turelli 1989; Wilkinson et al. 1990; Beniwal et al. 1992; Shaw et al. 1995). Second, a population in the neighborhood of a single adaptive peak will eventually climb the peak regardless of the pattern of genetic (co)variances (Lande 1979; Via and Lande 1985; Zeng 1988), in which case the role of quantitative genetics is only temporary. This second argument assumes that genetic constraints are not severe and that only a single fitness optimum is present. Quantitative genetic effects on the direction of evolution may endure when genetic variance in some directions is completely lacking (Gomulkiewicz and Kirkpatrick 1992) or when the selection surface has several local optima (Lande 1979; Arnold 1992; Price et al. 1993).

Here I take an empirical approach and ask whether, and for what period of time, the direction and extent of morphological diversification is influenced by genetic variances and covariances between the traits. I focus on a simple index of genetic variance and covariance within populations: the multivariate direction of greatest genetic variation. By "direction" I mean a vector of coefficients that describe a linear combination of the original morphological traits. The linear combination having maximum genetic variance within a population defines the genetic "line of least resistance" to evolutionary change by natural selection (Stebbins 1974; see Futuyma et al. 1993).

Three predictions are tested. The first is that populations and species very recently diverged from a common ancestor should differ in a direction close to the direction of greatest genetic variance (hereafter [g.sub.max]). This prediction stems from the fact that a population under the influence of a single fitness optimum does not evolve in the direction of greatest fitness increase (Lande 1979; Via and Lande 1985). Instead, progress toward an optimum should follow a curved trajectory that, in its initial stages, follows the direction of greatest genetic variance [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1 OMITTED]. Therefore, the direction of the line separating species means (hereafter z) should be biased toward [g.sub.max] when species are very closely related. I quantify the bias using the angle [Theta] between [g.sub.max] and z.

The predicted angle [Theta] depends also on the location of the optimum, which is unknown. However, if the direction of selection is random with respect to the orientation of [g.sub.max] then there should still be a tendency for initial divergence between populations and species to follow [g.sub.max]. In the Discussion I consider the possibility that optima are not randomly oriented with respect to [g.sub.max]. I also consider the consequences of multiple optima.

The second prediction is that this bias to the direction of evolution should be temporary and diminish with time. This is illustrated in Figure 1 by the tendency for the direction of difference between species, z, to move farther away from [g.sub.max] at progressively later stages of divergence. This decay in the value of [Theta] with time is expected even if genetic covariances remain constant, but it may be hastened if the covariances change as well (for simplicity I will use "covariances" to refer to both genetic variances and covariances). The duration of the bias to the direction of evolution imparted by genetic parameters has not been previously estimated.

The third prediction is that progress should be relatively slow if selection favors divergence in a direction markedly different from that of [g.sub.max], at least in the initial stages of divergence. This is because genetic variance is relatively low in directions other than [g.sub.max], and this reduces the amount of evolutionary response expected from a given intensity of directional selection (Lande 1979). None of these predictions assumes that genetic covariances are constant through time and in all species. Indeed, malleable covariances may be an additional reason to expect that their influence will be temporary.

To test these predictions, I measured quantitative genetic variation in five ecologically important morphological traits in a threespine stickleback Gasterosteus sp. The species is part of a recent adaptive radiation of sticklebacks in freshwater lakes and streams of coastal British Columbia, Canada (Schluter and McPhail 1992; McPhail 1993; Schluter 1996; Taylor et al., in press). I compare [g.sub.max] within the stickleback population to the direction of differences among populations in the same five traits. I then repeat this analysis on species in four other vertebrate taxa using data from the literature. This broader comparison was restricted to vertebrates because the ecological relevance of the morphological traits measured is known or suspected; this would increase the likelihood that natural selection (rather than drift) was the cause of evolutionary divergence. Finally, I quantify the resemblance between [g.sub.max], the direction of greatest genetic variance, and the direction of greatest phenotypic variance within populations to determine whether the latter yields similar results.

METHODS

Threespine Sticklebacks

Genetic parameters were estimated in the "limnetic" species of threespine stickleback from Enos Lake (Gasterosteus sp.), Vancouver Island (McPhail 1984; Schluter and McPhail 1992). This freshwater species is one of many derived from the cosmopolitan marine species (G. aculeatus) that colonized newly formed lakes and rivers at the end of the Pleistocene (McPhail 1993; Taylor et al., in press). The Enos Lake limnetic is sympatric with and reproductively isolated from a second species, the "benthic" (McPhail 1984; Ridgway and McPhail 1984; Schluter and McPhail 1992). The two forms are morphologically distinct and exploit different habitats. The traits that distinguish them strongly affect feeding efficiency and growth rate in the different habitats (Schluter 1993, 1995).

Genetic parameters were estimated from measurements of wild-caught parents and their lab-reared offspring. Crosses were made in the field by extracting and mixing eggs and sperm from randomly paired adults captured in traps and nets. Parents were then fixed in 10% formalin and later stained with alizarin red and transferred to 37% isopropyl alcohol. Fertilized eggs from a total of 67 families were transported to the lab, where they were raised in 10-liter aquaria. Offspring were fed once daily infusoria culture, brine shrimp nauplii, and later, tubilex worms. After three months, one to five offspring from each family were selected at random, preserved, and stained in the manner of their parents.

Five morphological traits were measured as described in Schluter and McPhail (1992): body length, body depth, gape width, gill raker number, and gill raker length. Measurements were ln transformed and size-corrected as described in detail in Schluter and McPhail (1992). Measurements from sibs were then averaged to yield a single offspring mean per family. Additive genetic variances and covariances between traits were estimated as twice the observed covariance between trait values in offspring and midparents (Falconer 1989). This method yielded two estimates of genetic covariance between each pair of traits, which were then averaged. Because parents and offspring were raised in different environments, I cannot rule out the possibility that estimates of genetic covariance are biased by genotype-environment interactions (Riska et al. 1989).

The direction of greatest genetic variation ([g.sub.max]) is the major axis (first principal component or dominant eigenvector) of the resulting genetic covariance matrix (Pimentel 1979; Johnson and Wichern 1982). Figure 1 illustrates [g.sub.max] in two dimensions as the longest axis of the ellipse describing the distribution of individual genetic values. The elements (coefficients) of [g.sub.max] measure the contributions of each of the five traits to the most variable direction. For example, the coefficient for trait 1 in Figure 1 is greater than that for trait 2, because a unit change in the direction of [g.sub.max] involves a greater displacement along the x-axis than along the y-axis. These coefficients were scaled such that [[g.sub.max]][prime][[g.sub.max]] = 1 (where [prime] indicates transpose). Total additive genetic variance in the population is the sum of the genetic variances of the five traits. Genetic variance among individuals in the direction [g.sub.max] made up 71% of this total.

Populations included in the analysis were those presented in Schluter and McPhail (1992). In the other vertebrate data sets I analyzed, I calculated z as simply the direction of the line connecting species means (see Other Vertebrates for the calculation and Fig. 1 for an illustration). I modified this step in the sticklebacks to accommodate a large number of populations whose phylogenetic relationships are still obscure. Rather than calculate the direction of the difference between each pair of population means, I calculated z as the single major axis of variation among all the population means. This measurement is roughly the same as the average of the directions between all pairs of populations. The elements of z were scaled so that z[prime]z = 1. The angle between [g.sub.max] and z was calculated as [Theta] = [cos.sup.-1] [([g.sub.max])[prime]z] (Pimentel 1979). The major axis z accounted for 80% of the total variance among population means.

I tested the null hypothesis that in sticklebacks [Theta] = 0 using the following bootstrap procedure: (1) 67 families of sticklebacks (parents and offspring) were randomly sampled with replacement from the original pool of 67 families ("with replacement" results in some families being chosen more than once and other families not chosen); (2) a new value of [g.sub.max] was computed from this "new" sample and its angle [Theta] from the original z was computed; (3) steps 1-2 were repeated 1000 times. The fraction of iterations in which the new angle exceeded the original [Theta] is an approximate P-value for a test of the null hypothesis that [Theta] = 0 (Efron and Tibshirani 1986). The standard deviation of the 1000 resampled vectors is the standard error of the estimated coefficients of [g.sub.max]. The reliability of the bootstrap method has not been evaluated in this context, but it is expected to behave well when, as in the present case, sample size is reasonable and the data are nearly normally distributed (Efron and Tibshirani 1986). This test assumes that the among-population direction is estimated without error. The null hypothesis that [Theta] = 0 would be more difficult to reject if sampling error in z were also incorporated.

Other Vertebrates

Published genetic covariances for ecologically relevant morphological traits were obtained for the Galapagos medium ground finch Geospiza fortis (Boag 1983), song sparrow Melospiza melodia (Schluter and Smith 1986a), collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis (Merila et al. 1994), and Peromyscus mice (Lofsvold 1986). (Co)variances were based on ln-transformed traits in most cases; in the remaining cases I used the squared coefficient of covariation instead.

In each taxon, the species for which [g.sub.max] was measured was designated the focal species ("ancestral" species in [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1 OMITTED]). The direction of difference z was then measured between the focal species and each species closely related to it. Each [z.sub.i] was calculated as [Mathematical Expression Omitted], where [Mathematical Expression Omitted] and [Mathematical Expression Omitted] are the vectors of mean measurements for the focal species and the given related species i, respectively. The quantity between parentheses on the right-hand side of this equation is the inverse of the morphological (euclidean) distance between the pair of population means, which scales the direction so that each [z[prime].sub.i][z.sub.i] = 1. The number of other species with which the focal species was compared was determined by the availabilities of morphological data and allozyme (or other molecular) measurements of relatedness.

Biochemical measures of relatedness were available for G. fortis and 10 other Galapagos finch species (Yang and Patton 1981). Five morphological traits were measured on these species (Grant et al. 1985): wing length, tarsus length, beak length, depth, and width. A species mean is the average of population (island) means. Natural selection occurs frequently on the five traits in every population that has been studied in detail (Boag and Grant 1981; Price et al. 1984; Gibbs and Grant 1987; Grant and Grant 1989) and species differences in morphology are strongly correlated with differences in diet (Grant 1986; Grant and Grant 1989). Natural selection was doubtless the main cause of morphological differentiation in this group.

Similar data were available for the song sparrow and its relatives. Allozyme distances between the song sparrow and nine species of Melospiza, Zonotrichia, and Junco were taken from Zink (1982). The same five morphological traits were measured in these species as in the Galapagos finches (Schluter and Smith 1986b; Schluter 1986, 1988a). Natural selection was observed to act frequently on these traits in a population of song sparrow studied intensively by J. N. M. Smith (Schluter and Smith 1986b; Schluter 1988b). This, along with the known association between these traits and feeding ecology in birds in general (Miles and Ricklefs 1984; Grant 1986; Miles et al. 1987; Richman and Price 1992; Suhonen et al. 1994), support the assumption that selection is the cause of their divergence.

In the collared flycatcher, [g.sub.max] is compared with the line of differentiation between this species and the closely related pied flycatcher, F. hypoleuca. Seven morphological traits were measured in both species: beak length, depth, width, tarsus length, length of the wing, tail, and first primary feather (Merila et al. 1994). There is no direct evidence of natural selection on these traits in the two flycatchers. However, most of these traits are known to be associated with feeding ecology and habitat in birds in general (Miles and Ricklefs 1984; Grant 1986; Miles et al. 1987; Richman and Price 1992; Suhonen et al. 1994), and I will assume that natural selection is responsible for morphological divergence between species (Merila et al. 1994). Note that the number of traits measured in the flycatchers (seven) is two more than that available for sticklebacks, finches, and sparrows. This means that the results would not be directly comparable because the angle [Theta] between [g.sub.max] and z is expected to depend on the number of traits. To solve this problem, I computed [Theta] for all subsets of five traits in the flycatchers and used the average. An allozyme-based measure of relatedness between the two species was found in Gelter et al. (1989).

Measurement means for two subspecies of Peromyscus maniculatus and one of P. leucopus were kindly provided by D. Lofsvold (unpubl. data). Two independent comparisons of means were therefore made: one between the two subspecies of P. maniculatus and the other between the average of these two subspecies and P. leucopus. The traits were head length, body length, tail length, ear length, several dimensions of the skull, mandible length, and molar occlusal surface area. The functional basis of variation in these traits is not well known and the assumption that natural selection is responsible for differences between populations and species studied is therefore the least supported of my data sets. However, Lofsvold (1988) showed that covariances among population means were not proportional to genetic covariances within the populations, ruling out genetic drift as a cause of differentiation. Also, some associations between limb and tail measurements and resource use have been discovered (Horner 1954). I therefore decided to include Peromyscus in the present survey.

Estimates of genetic covariance of traits were available for all three populations of Peromyscus (Lofsvold 1986) and I pooled estimates of the forms involved in each particular comparison. Fifteen traits were measured. To compare findings with the other data sets, I carried out analyses on all subsets of five traits and averaged the results. Allozyme measurements of relatedness between the three forms were taken from Avise et al. (1979).

I used Nei's (1978) genetic distance calculated from allozyme frequencies to measure the time separating a pair of species. Rogers's (1972) distance was strongly correlated with Nei's in these same groups (r = 0.99, t = 28.82, df = 17, P [less than] 0.001; tested using general least squares - see following section for explanation) and it led to identical results. For sticklebacks I used the average allozyme distance between freshwater populations in the region (Withler and McPhail 1985; McPhail 1984, 1992).

Statistical Analysis

I used linear regression to compare [Theta] with other variables such as the amount of evolutionary differentiation between species and the time (allozyme distance) separating them. However, separate observations within the sparrow and finch clades are not statistically independent because of shared history. For example, the sparrows provide nine measurements of z (yielding nine measurements of [Theta]), but each of them is calculated as the difference between the song sparrow and one of nine relatives. Similarly, z is measured between G. fortis and each of 10 other Galapagos finch species. The advantage of this approach is that every [Theta] can be calculated using [g.sub.max] of the focal species (the only species for which genetic parameters are known). An alternative approach based on independent contrasts (Felsenstein 1985; Harvey and Pagel 1991) would require the additional assumption that [g.sub.max] of at least one of the species involved in each contrast is the same as [g.sub.max] of the focal species.

I used general least squares (GLS) to solve the problem of nonindependent observations (Kendall and Stuart 1979; Draper and Smith 1981). This is a form of weighted regression that incorporates a matrix V whose elements [v.sub.i,j] are "correlations" specifying the degree of dependence between each pair of species i and j. The upshot of the approach is that a large value for an element of V assigns a low weight to the corresponding observation. The GLS model is:

[Y.sub.c] = [X.sub.c][Beta] + [[Epsilon].sub.c]. (1)

[X.sub.c] and [Y.sub.c] are transformed X and Y variables: [Y.sub.c] = [V.sup.-1/2] Y and [X.sub.c] = [V.sup.-1/2] X. [V.sup.-1/2] is the inverse of the "square root" of V (i.e., [[V.sup.1/2]][prime][V.sup.1/2] = V). [[Epsilon].sub.c] is the vector of residuals. This transformation corrects X and Y so that they fulfill the regression requirement of independent observations and homogeneous variances of residuals. The regression coefficients [Beta] and their standard errors are then estimated in the usual way (least squares). GLS has been used in previous studies to correct measurements that are nonindependent because of phylogenetic relationships between species (Lynch 1991; Lynch and Jarrell 1993).

The matrix V was constructed as a series of submatrices representing individual clades in the data set. Within a clade the degree of dependence [v.sub.i,j] between any pair of species i and j was calculated from branch lengths in the tree of phylogenetic relationships. For example, the phylogeny for sparrows (Zink 1982) was first redrawn such that the focal species (song sparrow) was the root of the tree, while retaining the original branch lengths. The expected dependence between observations for any two sparrow species i and j was then calculated as the branch length shared between these two species in the paths leading from each of them to the root of the tree. When i = j, branch length shared is simply the sum of branch lengths between the given species i and the root of the tree.

Because branch length is in arbitrary units of Nei's (1978) allozyme distance, I scaled values of [v.sub.i,j] by dividing them by the smallest value (summed branch length) in the given sub-matrix (dividing by the largest value instead gave virtually identical results). Submatrices for clades with only a single observation (sticklebacks and flycatchers) or where different values are independent (mice) were given 1s along the diagonal and 0s elsewhere. Submatrices were then arranged along the diagonal of V. The dependence between observations from different submatrices (clades) was assumed to be zero.

GLS was also used to test whether the observed mean [Theta], based on all measurements combined, was significantly different from the random expectation of 1.18 radians (about 68 [degrees]). This expected angle was calculated as the mean of angles between a large number of random vectors and an arbitrary fixed vector (I used the unit vector [0, 0, 0, 0, 1], but the result is the same with any other vector). I created each random vector by breaking a stick of length 1 into five pieces, with the break points drawn from a uniform random number generator. Each element of the random vector was the length of the corresponding piece of the broken stick. The angle between this vector and the fixed vector was then calculated. This procedure was repeated ten thousand times.

All calculations and statistical analyses were carried out using the computer program S-plus 3.1 (Statistical Sciences 1992).

RESULTS

Sticklebacks

The five morphological traits were heritable and genetically intercorrelated (Table 1). The direction of greatest genetic variance ([g.sub.max]; Table 2) showed that genotypes at one extreme of the population had relatively short, slender bodies; narrow mouths; and numerous, long gill rakers. Genotypes at the other extreme had the opposite set of features: relatively [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] long, deep bodies; wide mouths; and few, short gill rakers. This direction of maximum genetic variance within populations ([g.sub.max]) was surprisingly similar to the direction of differences among population means (Table 2). The angle [Theta] between [g.sub.max] and z was small (0.31 radians, about 18 [degrees], which was not significantly different from 0; Table 2). This [Theta] is also well below the random expectation of 1.18 radians (bootstrap P [less than] 0.001). These young stickleback populations and species have diverged mainly along [g.sub.max], the genetic line of least resistance.

Overall Trends

Data from other available vertebrate groups confirms the pattern seen in the sticklebacks. The vast majority of evolution is in a direction closer to [g.sub.max] than expected by chance [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]. The mean angle in this data set was estimated as [Mathematical Expression Omitted] radians [+ or -]0.13 SE, which is well below the random expectation of 1.18 radians (GLS; t = -4.244, df = 22, P = 0.0002). The results confirm the expectation that evolution is biased toward [g.sub.max] among species that have diverged relatively recently. Values for [Theta] are nevertheless highly variable and several values approach or exceed the random expectation [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]. For example, divergence between the two species of flycatchers was in a direction very different from [g.sub.max].

The smallest values of [Theta] tended to occur between the most recently diverged species. This was true of species in four of five taxa whose date of divergence was less than about 0.07 allozyme distance units (flycatchers are the exception; [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]). More distantly related species tended to differ in a direction closer on average to the random expectation. This tendency was also present within each of the three taxa represented by more than one observation (sparrows, Galapagos finches, and mice), though the trend is slight in the third case. These patterns hint that the genetic bias to the direction of evolution may decay with time. However, the positive overall relationship between [Theta] and time was not significant (GLS; [Mathematical Expression Omitted], t = 1.157, df = 21, P = 0.13, one-tailed test) because of the high variability.
TABLE 2. Direction of greatest genetic variance [g.sub.max] within
the focal stickleback population and the direction z of highest
variability among population means; [g.sub.max] is the first
principal component of the covariance matrix given in Table 1. SEs
for the coefficients of [g.sub.max] (in parentheses) were
calculated
by bootstrapping. Bootstrapping additionally showed that
[g.sub.max]
is significantly different from all five unit vectors (e.g.,
[0,0,0,0,1]; all five P [less than] 0.01) indicating that the five
traits are significantly genetically intercorrelated. The angle
between the two vectors ([Theta] = 0.31 radians) was not
significantly different from zero (bootstrap P = 0.11; tested
assuming that the among-population direction is estimated without
sampling error).

Trait                   [g.sub.max]                   Z

Length                 -0.21 (0.07)                 -0.14
Depth                  -0.06 (0.09)                 -0.35
Gape width             -0.59 (0.08)                 -0.47
Raker number            0.72 (0.05)                  0.75
Raker length            0.29 (0.12)                  0.29


The amount of evolutionary divergence between species was inversely related to 0 [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 3 OMITTED], in accord with the third prediction. That is, the greater the departure between the direction of evolution and the direction of greatest genetic variance, the smaller the amount of morphological change (GLS; [Mathematical Expression Omitted], t = -3.851, df = 21, P = 0.0005, one-tailed test). The flycatchers exemplify the pattern: their direction of divergence was greatly different from [g.sub.max] but their progress was small [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 3 OMITTED]. This analysis does not correct for the ages of the different species represented, but the result was similar when I used residuals from a regression of morphological distance on time instead (GLS; [Mathematical Expression Omitted], t = -3.333, df = 21, P = 0.0016, one-tailed test).

Duration of Effect

These results demonstrate that the course of adaptive diversification is biased by genetic covariances within populations. The question I now address is: how long does this effect typically last?

The bias to the direction of evolution was strongest among the most closely related species and tended to decay with time [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]. If this tendency is real, then these data suggest that the bias endures until species are at least 0.3 units of Nei's distance apart. In passerine birds, 0.1 units of Nei's distance corresponds to roughly 3% sequence divergence in mitochondrial DNA, or about 1.5 million years (Zink 1991). An allozyme distance of nearly 0.3 between the two species of mice (P. maniculatus and P. leucopus) [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED] also corresponds to a mtDNA sequence divergence of roughly 10% (extrapolated from data in Avise et al. 1979 and DeWalt et el. 1993). On this scale, 0.3 distance units would represent over four million years. This estimate is conservative because the trend suggested in Figure 2 may be spurious (i.e., it is not statistically significant). The bias to the direction of evolution may therefore last longer than the four million years spanned by the present data.

The effect of [g.sub.max] on the absolute amount of evolutionary divergence showed no tendency to weaken with time; absolute divergence was inversely related to [Theta] across the full range of species ages [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 3 OMITTED]. The same was true of evolutionary rate (residuals from a regression of morphological distance on time). There were no examples in these data of a large amount of evolution in a direction markedly different from [g.sub.max]. Therefore, the influence of genetic variance and covariance on the rate and amount of morphological change endures for longer than four million years.

Phenotypic versus Genetic Lines of Least Resistance

Genetic covariances are difficult and costly to measure and published examples from vertebrates remain few - particularly of traits thought to have diverged by natural selection rather than genetic drift. This dearth limits the strength of some results, especially estimates of the temporal decay of [Theta]. Here I ask whether the direction of greatest phenotypic variance, [p.sub.max], is similar to [g.sub.max], the direction of greatest genetic variance. If so, then it may be possible to substitute [p.sub.max] for [g.sub.max] in future analyses of this kind, greatly increasing the potential sample size. Similarity between [g.sub.max] and [p.sub.max] was measured by the angle between them (I used the average angle of all subsets of five traits when more than five traits were measured).

Similarity between [g.sub.max] and [p.sub.max] varied greatly among taxa. The difference between them was negligible in the Galapagos finch (0.08 radians, 4.5 [degrees]) and small to moderate in the stickleback (0.28 radians, 16 [degrees]), mouse (0.31 radians, 18 [degrees]; calculated after pooling the three covariance matrices available), flycatcher (0.46 radians, 27 [degrees]), and sparrow (0.60 radians, 35 [degrees]). These angles between [g.sub.max] and [p.sub.max] are all well below the random expectation of 1.18 radians (68 [degrees]). Data are not available in most cases to test whether these angles between [g.sub.max] and [p.sub.max] are significantly different from zero.

Despite these differences, [g.sub.max] and [p.sub.max] were nearly equally successful at predicting the direction of divergence between species. The average angle between [p.sub.max] and the direction of species divergence was 0.63 [+ or -] 0.14 SE, which is hardly larger than [Mathematical Expression Omitted] (0.61 [+ or -] 0.13 SE). The biggest difference between any individual value of [Theta] [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED] and the same angle computed using [p.sub.max] was 0.19 radians (11 [degrees]). This suggests that [p.sub.max] might be a reasonable substitute for [g.sub.max] in many instances, for comparisons with directions of species divergence. This does not contradict Willis et el. (1991), who argued that phenotypic covariances are not good surrogates for genetic covariances when making precise predictions of evolutionary change across a generation. Phenotypic data may, however, be adequate for the coarser kinds of comparisons made here.

DISCUSSION

Despite widespread appreciation of the role of genetic (including developmental) constraints in evolution, quantitative tests are scarce. A few studies have shown that rate of evolution (but not necessarily its direction) is predicted by patterns of genetic variance. In an earlier study, I showed that the size of the morphological gap between Galapagos finch species was a poorer indicator of their phylogenetic affinity than was the net intensity of selection needed to cross the gap, which takes into account genetic covariances between traits (Schluter 1984); it follows that rates of divergence between species were limited by genetic covariance. Kluge and Kerfoot (1973) suggested that patterns of phenotypic variation within species, which might be a reasonable surrogate for genetic variation (Cheverud 1988; but see Willis et al. 1991), predicted amounts of morphological variation among species (see also Olson and Miller 1958; but see Rohlf et al. 1983).

Few studies have tested for an effect of genetic constraints on the direction of evolution. Futuyma et al. (1993, 1995) found that genetic variation in feeding response and survival of Ophraella leaf beetles on different host plants was frequently lacking and that this had partly limited the kinds of host shifts that occurred during the diversification of the genus. Phenotypic variation induced by manipulating amphibian development partly predicted the pattern of morphological differences seen among species (Alberch and Gale 1985). Theoretical models of morphogenesis and pattern formation have shown that relatively simple changes in development can produce observed patterns of species differences (Oster et al. 1988; Nijhout 1991; Price and Pavelka, in press).

The evidence presented here is therefore among the first to show that patterns of quantitative genetic covariance bias the direction of evolution over reasonably long spans of time. It also shows that amounts of morphological divergence in particular directions varies with the amount of genetic variance in those directions. The results provide a preliminary estimate of the duration of genetic constraints on morphological evolution. My study has been limited to functionally important traits in vertebrates on which selection has been directly recorded in some cases. The implication is that for at least four million years evolution in functionally important traits was most frequent, and evolutionary change greatest, along genetic lines of least resistance. Results also indicate that, for these data sets, the direction of greatest phenotypic variance is a reasonable substitute for [g.sub.max], the direction of greatest genetic variance.

Genetic constraints on evolution is a reasonable explanation for these findings. Figure 1 illustrates how this may happen. The scenario is simplistic, however, because it implies that species are under continuous directional selection. In contrast, field studies suggest that stabilizing selection and/or oscillating directional selection is more common (Schluter and Smith 1986b; Gibbs and Grant 1987). An alternative scenario in which the adaptive landscape is contoured with many fitness peaks, rather than just one, and in which populations infrequently move between peaks may be more realistic than Figure 1. In this case the predicted trajectory will depend on the nature of the landscape, but the coarse features of divergence should be similar to those expected under the simple scenario. A population in a new environment temporarily under the influence of two (or more) adaptive peaks should tend to evolve toward that peak whose direction from the population mean is closest to [g.sub.max] (Lande 1979; Burger 1986; Arnold 1992; Price et al. 1993). This effect should also decay with time, as the number of adaptive peak shifts separating any two species increases. The qualitative effect of genetic constraints on evolutionary direction may be robust to features of the selection surface, provided that the location of available adaptive peaks is unbiased with respect to the direction of [g.sub.max].

Two alternative explanations, however, may also account for the results. The first is that directional selection for species divergence is not random with respect to the orientation of [g.sub.max], but instead tends to be strongest along it. Under this hypothesis [g.sub.max] predicts the direction and rate of evolution because it is molded by these same natural selection pressures. For example, the presence of two resources (e.g., plankton and benthos in a lake) may subject a population exploiting them to correlational selection if each resource is best exploited by individuals having a distinct combination of trait values (e.g., many gill rakers and a narrow mouth, or few gill rakers and a wide mouth; cf. Table 1). Patterns of genetic covariance may then evolve to conform more closely to the pattern of selection (Lande 1980; Cheverud 1984; but see Turelli 1985). If species divergence then results in a partitioning of the two resources, morphological differences between the new species will match the genetic extremes of the prior populations, and hence will occur along the line of maximum genetic variance ([g.sub.max]). Low [Theta] in this case is caused not by genetic constraint but by genetic "pre-destination."

This alternative hypothesis assumes that genetic covariances evolve in an adaptive way to conform to the shape of the selection surface. Whether this can happen is highly debatable (Wilkinson et al. 1990; Arnold 1992). A test of the assumption would require that [g.sub.max] be compared among populations and species inhabiting different environments with contrasting patterns of selection; the assumption is rejected if [g.sub.max] does not vary as predicted. I am aware of only one study that has compared genetic covariances between populations inhabiting environments with known differences in selection regime. Genetic correlations between eye and antennal characters in the amphipod Gammarus minus were stronger in both of two cave populations measured (where small eyes and large antennae are favored) than in two spring populations (where eyes are larger and antennae are favored) (Jernigan et al. 1994). This reveals that genetic correlations are somewhat malleable under selection, but it was not determined whether the differences in genetic correlations between cave and spring populations were adaptive.

A second alternative is that genetic covariances are shaped in part by gene flow between species and this causes [g.sub.max] to conform to the direction of species differences. One way in which this might happen is through ongoing hybridization between species with different mean values for a suite of traits. Such hybridization maintains genetic variance in the direction of the difference between species by creating linkage disequilibrium (nonrandom associations) between alleles at the different loci responsible for species differences. Another way is through occasional movement from one species to another of alleles with pleiotropic effects on numerous traits. Linkage disequilibrium breaks down extremely rapidly once gene flow has ceased, but alleles with pleiotropic effects could potentially linger for some time after (T. D. Price, pers. comm., 1994).

I consider this second alternative less likely than the others for the following reasons. Not all species in taxa sampled hybridize currently, for example, the focal sparrow species does not (Schluter and Smith 1986a; gene flow between geographically differentiated song sparrow populations could, however, produce the same effect if the morphological axis of population differentiation is similar to that between closely related species). The stickleback species used here (Enos Lake limnetic) possesses a unique allele that is not present in its sympatric congener (McPhail 1984), suggesting that gene flow is low (but unidirectional gene flow from the benthic to the limnetic is not ruled out by this observation). Hybridization also does not ensure that [g.sub.max] will be affected. For example, the two flycatcher species hybridize (Gelter et al. 1989, 1992; this may also have reduced the allozyme distance between them and hence their estimated age), but [g.sub.max] remains very different from the line of difference between species means [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]. Finally, [g.sub.max] in the Galapagos ground finches is similar to the line of maximum phenotypic variance ([p.sub.max]), which is an axis of general body size (Boag 1983; Schluter 1984). Yet [p.sub.max] tends to be similar in all ground finch species whether they hybridize or not (Schluter 1984, unpubl. data; Grant et al. 1985).

These alternative explanations need further elaboration and testing. Nevertheless, despite uncertainty over causes, the results presented here show that estimates of quantitative genetic parameters are useful over extended periods of evolutionary time, even when change occurs by natural selection. Measures of genetic covariance are therefore valuable when the goal is to predict trends in the direction of adaptive differentiation.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank J. D. McPhail, R. Gullison, and R. Repasky for help when making and rearing the crosses; R. Gullison and Starbucks for carefully measuring all the fish; and J. D. McPhail for inspiring much of the stickleback work. D. Lofsvold kindly provided measurement means for the Peromyscus populations. L. Gustafsson sent a timely preprint of his flycatcher work. Thanks to P. Grant, D. Futuyma, T. Hatfield, S. Heard, T. Price, J. Pritchard, M. Turelli and an anonymous reviewer for their comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (Canada).

LITERATURE CITED

ALBERCH, P., AND E. A. GALE. 1985. A developmental analysis of an evolutionary trend: Digital reduction in amphibians. Evolution 39:8-23.

ARNOLD, S. J. 1992. Constraints on phenotypic evolution. Am. Nat. 140 (Suppl.):S85-S107.

AVISE, J. C., M. H. SMITH, AND R. K. SELANDER. 1979. Biochemical polymorphism and systematics in the genus Peromyscus. VII. Geographic differentiation in members of the truei and maniculatis species groups. J. Mammal. 60:177-192.

BARTON, N.H., AND M. TURELLI. 1989. Evolutionary quantitative genetics - How little do we know. Annu. Rev. Genet. 23:337-370.

BENIWAL, B. K., I. M. HASTINGS, R. THOMPSON, AND W. G. HILL. 1992. Estimation of changes in genetic parameters in selected lines using REML with the animal model. 1. Body weight, body composition, and litter size. Heredity 69:361-371.

BOAG, P. T. 1983. The heritability of external morphology in the Darwin's finches (Geospizinae) of Daphne Major Island, Galapagos. Evolution 37:877-894.

BOAG, P. T., AND P. R. GRANT. 1981. Intense natural selection in a population of Darwin's finches (Geospizinae) in the Galapagos. Science 214:82-85.

BONNER, J. T., ED. 1982. Evolution and development. Springer, Berlin.

BURGER, R. 1986. Constraints for the evolution of functionally coupled characters: A nonlinear analysis of a phenotypic model. Evolution 40:182-193.

CHEVERUD, J. M. 1984. Quantitative genetics and developmental constraints on evolution by selection. J. Theoret. Biol. 110:155-171.

-----. 1988. A comparison of genetic and phenotypic correlations. Evolution 42:958-968.

DEWALT, T. S., E.G. ZIMMERMAN, AND J. V. PLANZ. 1993. Mitochondrial-DNA phylogeny of species of the boylii and truei groups of the genus Peromyscus. J. Mammal. 74:352-362.

DRAPER, N., AND H. SMITH. 1981. Applied regression analysis. 2d ed. Wiley, Chichester, UK.

EFRON, B., AND R. TIBSHIRANI. 1986. Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Stat. Sci. 1:54-77.

FALCONER, D. S. 1989. Introduction to quantitative genetics. 3d ed. Longman, New York.

FELSENSTEIN, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat. 125:1-15.

FUTUYMA, D. J. 1988. Sturm und Drang and the evolutionary synthesis. Evolution 42:217-226.

FUTUYMA, D. J., M. C. KEESE, AND S. J. SCHEFFER. 1993. Genetic constraints and the phylogeny of insect-plant associations: Responses of Ophraella communa (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae) to host plants of its congeners. Evolution 47:888-905.

FUTUYMA, D. J., M. C. KEESE, AND D. J. FUNK. 1995. Genetic constraints on macroevolution: The evolution of host affiliation in the leaf beetle genus Ophraella. Evolution:49:797-809.

GELTER, H. P., H. TEGELSTROM, AND G. STAHL. 1989. Allozyme similarity between the pied and collared flycatchers (Aves, Ficedula hypoleuca and F. albicollis). Hereditas 111:65-72.

GELTER, H. P., H. TEGELSTROM, AND L. GUSTAFSSON. 1992. Evidence from hatching success and DNA fingerprinting for the fertility of hybrid pied x collared flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca x albicollis. Ibis 134:62-68.

GIBBS, H. L., AND P. R. GRANT. 1987. Oscillating selection on Darwin's finches. Nature 327:511-513.

GOMULKIEWICZ, R., AND M. KIRKPATRICK. 1992. Quantitative genetics and the evolution of reaction norms. Evolution 46:390-411.

GRANT, B. R., AND P. R. GRANT. 1989. Evolutionary dynamics of a natural population. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.

-----. 1993. Evolution of Darwin finches caused by a rare climatic event. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 251:111-117.

GRANT, P. R. 1986. Ecology and evolution of Darwin's finches. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ.

GRANT, P. R., AND B. R. GRANT. 1995. Predicting microevolutionary responses to directional selection on heritable variation. Evolution 49:241-251.

GRANT, P. R., I. ABBOTT, D. SCHLUTER, R. L. CURRY, AND L. K. ABBOTT. 1985. Variation in the size and shape of Darwin's finches. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 25:1-39.

HARVEY, P. H., AND M. D. PAGEL. 1991. The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford.

HORNER, E. B. 1954. Arboreal adaptations of Peromyscus, with special reference to the use of the tail. Contrib. Lab. Vert. Biol. Univ. Mich. 61:1-85.

JERNIGAN, R. W., D. C. CULVER, AND D. W. FONG. 1994. The dual role of selection and evolutionary history as reflected in genetic correlations. Evolution 48:587-596.

JOHNSON, R. A., AND D. W. WICHERN. 1982. Applied multivariate statistical analysis. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

KENDALL, M. G., AND A. STUART. 1979. The advanced theory of statistics. Vol. 2. Inference and relationship. Griffin, London.

KLUGE, A. R., AND W. C. KERFOOT. 1973. The predictability and regularity of character divergence. Am. Nat. 107:426-442.

LANDE, R. 1979. Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: body size allometry. Evolution 33:402-416.

-----. 1980. The genetic covariance between characters maintained by pleiotropic mutations. Genetics 94:203-215.

LOFSVOLD, D. 1986. Quantitative genetics of morphological differentiation in Peromyscus. I. Test of the homogeneity of genetic covariance structure among species and subspecies. Evolution 40:559-573.

-----. 1988. Quantitative genetics of morphological differentiation in Peromyscus. II. Analysis of selection and drift. Evolution 42:54-67.

LYNCH, M. 1991. Methods for the analysis of comparative data in evolutionary biology. Evolution 45:1065-1080.

LYNCH, M., AND P. E. JARRELL. 1993. A method for calibrating molecular clocks and its application to animal mitochondrial DNA. Genetics 135:1197-1208.

MAYNARD SMITH, J, R. BURIAN, S. KAUFFMAN, P. ALBERCH, J. CAMPBELL, B. GOODWIN, R. LANDE, D. RAUP, AND L. WOLPERT. 1985. Developmental constraints and evolution. Q. Rev. Biol. 60:265-287.

MCPHAIL, J. D. 1984. Ecology and evolution of sympatric sticklebacks (Gasterosteus): Morphological and genetic evidence for a species pair in Enos Lake, British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 62:1402-1408.

-----. 1992. Ecology and evolution of sympatric sticklebacks (Gasterosteus): Evidence for a species pair in Paxton Lake, Texada Island, British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 70:361-369.

-----. 1993. Speciation and the evolution of reproductive isolation in the sticklebacks (Gasterosteus) of southwestern British Columbia. Pp. 399-437 in M. A. Bell and S. A. Foster, eds. Evolutionary biology of the threespine stickleback. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford.

MERILA, J., M. BJORKLUND, AND L. GUSTAFSSON. 1994. Evolution of morphological differences with moderate genetic correlations among traits as exemplified by two flycatcher species (Ficedula; Muscicapidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 52:19-30.

MILES, D. B., AND R. E. RICKLEFS. 1984. The correlation between ecology and morphology in deciduous forest passerine birds. Ecology 65:1629-1640.

MILES, D. B., R. E. RICKLEFS, AND J. TRAVIS. 1987. Concordance of ecomorphological relationships in three assemblages of passerine birds. Am. Nat. 129:347-364.

NEI, M. 1978. The theory of genetic distance and the evolution of human races. Japan. J. Hum. Genet. 23:341-369.

NIJHOUT, H. F. 1991. The development and evolution of butterfly wing patterns. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC.

OLSEN, E. C., AND R. L. MILLER. 1958. Morphological integration. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.

OSTER, G. F., N. SHUBIN, J. D. MURRAY, AND P. ALBERCH. 1988. Evolution and morphogenetic rules: The shape of the vertebrate limb in ontogeny and phylogeny. Evolution 42:862-884.

PIMENTEL, R. A. 1979. Morphometrics. Kendall/Hunt, Dubuque, IA.

PRICE, T., AND M. PAVELKA. In press. Evolution of a colour pattern: History, development, and selection. J. of Evol. Biol.

PRICE, T. D., P. R. GRANT, H. L. GIBBS, AND P. T. BOAG. 1984. Recurrent patterns of natural selection in a population of Darwin's finches. Nature 309:787-789.

PRICE, T., M. TURELLI, AND M. SLATKIN. 1993. Peak shifts produced by correlated response to selection. Evolution 47:280-290.

RICHMAN, A. D., AND T. PRICE. 1992. Evolution of ecological differences in the Old World leaf warblers, Nature: 355:817-821.

RIDGWAY, M. S., AND J. D. MCPHAIL. 1984. Ecology and evolution of sympatric sticklebacks (Gasterosteus): Mate choice and reproductive isolation in the Enos Lake species pair. Can. J. Zool. 62:1813-1818.

RISKA, B., T. PROUT, AND M. TURELLI. 1989. Laboratory estimates of heritabilities and genetic correlations in nature. Genetics 123:865-871.

ROGERS, J. S. 1972. Measures of genetic similarity and genetic distance. Univ. Texas Publ. 7213:145-153.

ROHLF, F. J., A. J. GILMARTIN, AND G. HART. 1983. The Kluge-Kerfoot phenomenon - A statistical artifact. Evolution 37:180-202.

SCHLUTER, D. 1984. Morphological and phylogenetic relations among the Darwin's finches. Evolution 38:921-930.

-----. 1986. Tests for similarity and convergence of finch communities. Ecology 67:1073-1085.

-----. 1988a. Character displacement and the adaptive divergence of finches on islands and continents. Am. Nat. 131:799-824.

-----. 1988b. Estimating the form of natural selection on a quantitative trait. Evolution 42:849-861.

-----. 1993. Adaptive radiation in sticklebacks: Size, shape and habitat use efficiency. Ecology 74:699-709.

-----. 1995. Adaptive radiation in sticklebacks: Trade-offs in feeding performance and growth. Ecology 76:82-90.

-----. 1996. Ecological causes of adaptive radiation. Am. Nat. 148(Suppl.):540-564.

SCHLUTER, D., AND J. D. MCPHAIL. 1992. Ecological character displacement and speciation in sticklebacks. Am. Nat. 140:85-108.

SCHLUTER, D., AND J. N. M. SMITH. 1986a. Genetic and phenotypic correlations in a natural population of song sparrows. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 29:23-36.

-----. 1986b. Natural selection on beak and body size in the song sparrow. Evolution 40:221-231.

SHAW, F. H., R. G. SHAW, G. S. WILKINSON, AND M. TURELLI. 1995. Changes in genetic variances and covariances: G whiz! Evolution 49:1260-1267.

STATISTICAL SCIENCES. 1992. S-plus. Statistical sciences, Seattle, WA.

STEBBINS, G. L. 1974. Flowering plants: Evolution above the species level. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA.

SUHONEN, J., R. V. ALATALO, AND L. GUSTAFSSON. 1994. Evolution of foraging ecology in Fennoscandian tits (Parus spp) Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 258:127-131.

TAYLOR, E. B., J. D. MCPHAIL, AND D. SCHLUTER. In press. History of ecological selection in sticklebacks: Uniting experimental and phylogenetic approaches. In T. J. Givnish and K. J. Sytsma, eds. Molecular evolution and adaptive radiation. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge.

TURELLI, M. 1985. Effects of pleiotropy on predictions concerning mutation selection balance for polygenic traits. Genetics 11:165-195.

-----. 1988. Phenotypic evolution, constant covariances, and the maintenance of addive variance. Evolution 42:1342-1348.

VIA, S., AND R. LANDE. 1985. Genotype-environment interaction and the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Evolution 39:505-522.

WAKE, D. B. 1991. Homoplasy: the result of natural selection or evidence of design limitations? Am. Nat. 138:543-567.

WILLIS, J. H., J. A. COYNE, AND M. KIRKPATRICK. 1991. Can one predict the evolution of quantitative characters without genetics? Evolution 45:441-444.

WILKINSON, G. S., K. FOWLER, AND L. PARTRIDGE. 1990. Resistance of genetic correlation structure to directional selection in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 44:1990-2003.

WITHLER, R. E., AND J. D. MCPHAIL. 1985. Genetic variability in freshwater and anadromous sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) of southern British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 63:528-533.

YANG, S. Y., AND J. L. PATTON. 1981. Genetic variability and differentiation in the Galapagos finches. Auk 98:230-242.

ZENG, Z.-B. 1988. Long-term correlated response, interpopulation covariation, and interspecific allometry. Evolution 42:363-374.

ZINK, R. M. 1982. Patterns of genic and morphological variation among sparrows in the genera Zonotrichia, Melozpiza, Junco, and Passerella. Auk 99:632-649.

-----. 1991. Concluding remarks: Modern biochemical approaches to avian systematics. Acta XX Congr. Int. Ornithol. Pp. 629-636 in New Zealand Ornithological Congress Trust Board, Wellington, New Zealand.
COPYRIGHT 1996 Society for the Study of Evolution
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1996 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Schluter, Dolph
Publication:Evolution
Date:Oct 1, 1996
Words:8301
Previous Article:Phylogenies, spatial autoregression, and the comparative method: a computer simulation test.
Next Article:Density-dependent selection on continuous characters: a quantitative genetic model.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2021 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters |