Printer Friendly

Activity and distribution of gray foxes (Urocyon cinereoargenteus) in southern California.

Risk of predation is a strong driver of mammalian behavior and can affect distribution and activity patterns of potential prey (Hebblewhite and Merrill, 2009; Sansom et al., 2009; Valeix et al., 2009; Anderson et al., 2010). Intraguild predation (Polis et al., 1989; Polis and Holt, 1992; Palomares and Caro, 1999) helps structure communities of predators and influences behaviors of both competing predators and their prey (Heithaus, 2001; Finke and Denno, 2002; Rosenheim, 2004). For sympatric canids, the threat of such predation appears to have important behavioral effects in a variety of communities (e.g., Gosselink et al., 2003; Switalski, 2003).

Activity patterns of gray foxes (Urocyon cinereoargenteus) may vary with temperature, season, activity of prey, or harassment from humans or other predators (Cypher, 2003). Similarly, local use of habitat by gray foxes often relates to foraging opportunities and to protection from intraguild predation (Cypher, 2003; Fuller and Cypher, 2004). During an investigation assessing competition among mesocarnivores in the Santa Monica Mountains of southern California, Fedriani et al. (2000) determined that gray foxes visited baited camera stations most often during the night-crepuscular period, similar to coyotes (Canis latrans), but more often than bobcats (Lynx rufus). This activity pattern was attributed to diet, with the proportion of diurnal squirrels being higher for bobcats, and mean overlap in diet being lowest for coyotes and gray foxes versus other combinations of species (Fedriani et al., 2000). In addition, limited data from cameras and live-trapping indicated a negative relationship between abundances of coyotes and gray foxes across habitats, suggesting that gray foxes avoided habitats where there was a high risk of predation by coyotes (Fedriani et al., 2000). This proposition is supported in one portion of the Santa Monica Mountains where 11 of 12 mortalities of radiomonitored gray foxes were due to predation by sympatric coyotes or bobcats (Farias et al., 2005). To more robustly document behavior of gray foxes in the presence of intraguild predators, we used radiotelemetry to investigate patterning of diel activity and use of habitats by gray foxes, coyotes, and bobcats. To reduce chances of attack, we hypothesized that gray foxes would exhibit activity patterns different from coyotes and bobcats, and that they would use habitats in different proportions compared to their sympatric predators.

MATERIALS AND METHODS--We studied a population of gray foxes living within the Simi Hills portion of Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area in Ventura and Los Angeles counties, which is adjacent to the metropolitan region of Los Angeles, California. The Simi Hills have large core areas of protected parkland surrounded by undeveloped private and public lands (Riley et al., 2003), and suburban developments that result from continuous fragmentation at the borders of the parkland (National Park Service, in litt.). More than 50,000 visitors/year frequent the park for activities such as hiking, mountain biking, and horse riding (National Park Service, in litt.). Elevation in the Simi Hills is 274-732 m (National Park Service, in litt.). Mild, wet winters (November-April) and hot, dry summers (May-October) characterize the Mediterranean climate of the Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area with annual minimum and maximum mean temperatures of 10.5 and 21.3[degrees]C, and annual mean precipitation of 376 mm occurring primarily as rains in winter. However, the meteorological phenomena El Nino and La Nina affected southern California during our study, producing dry seasons in 1997 and 1998, and 231% of average rainfall during the wet season in 1998 (National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, 1999).

Historically, grazing, fire, and urbanization have influenced distribution and composition of plant communities in the Simi Hills (National Park Service, in litt.). We characterized seven habitats in our study area: 1) northern mixed chaparral (34% of our study area) is a dense association of hard-leaved shrubs dominated by Ceanothus; 2) chamise chaparral (6%) is less dense than northern mixed chaparral and is dominated by Adenostoma fasciculata; 3) coastal sage scrub (40%) occurs as soft-leaved, grayish-green, aromatic shrubs (Artemisa, Salvia); 4) coast live oak (Quercus agrifolia) woodland (5%) has a dense overstory and can have a dense understory composed of woody species; 5) valley oak (Quercus lobata) woodland (<1%) produces less canopy cover than coast live oak and has a grass understory; 6) grassland (3%) is dominated by non-native annual grasses and forbs such as wild oat (Avena) and black mustard (Brassica nigra), and includes vestiges of native perennial bunchgrasses (Stipa, Elymus, Melica) and native annual grasses (Festuca); 7) development (11%) includes residential areas and human-influenced habitats on boundaries of the park.

Coyotes, bobcats, raccoons (Procyon lotor), and striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis) are other common species of carnivores in the study area, while spotted skunks (Spilogale putorius), long-tailed weasels (Mustela frenata), American badgers (Taxidea taxus), and cougars (Puma concolor) are rare locally (National Park Service, in litt.). Lagomorphs comprised the largest component of diets of gray foxes, coyotes, and bobcats in the Santa Monica Mountains (Fedriani et al., 2000), but various species of rodents also are important for each carnivore. In our study area, diets of coyotes and gray foxes were similar (seasonal overlap in foods was 0.52), with main differences being that coyotes consumed more fruits and nuts, and gray foxes ate more insects (Fedriani et al., 2000).

We trapped and radiocollared gray foxes during May-November 1997 and April-October 1998. To avoid injuring foxes, we used 1% coil-spring, soft-catch, leg-hold traps with padded jaws as used by Riley et al. (2003). We immobilized gray foxes by taping their muzzle and legs, and covering their eyes with a blindfold to reduce stress. We intramuscularly injected aggressive foxes with 5-10 mg/kg of ketamine hydrochloride following methods of Seal and Kreeger (1987). We attached numbered eartags and a 60-g radiocollar with a 20-cm whip antenna and mortality sensor (Lotek Wireless, Inc., Newmarket, Ontario, Canada, and Advanced Telemetry Systems, Isanti, Minnesota) to each fox. Subadults wore loosely fitted radio-collars to allow for growth. We recorded sex of foxes, estimated their age (subadults were <1 year old and adults were >1 year old) by irruption and wear of teeth and by size of body (mass, plus measurements of head, body, tail, hind foot, and ear were recorded as described by Fuller and Cypher, 2004), and then we released them at site of capture site.

We monitored and radiotracked gray foxes during May 1997-April 1999. Activity was treated as a dichotomous variable, i.e., active or inactive. To assess activity, we monitored radiosignals of individuals during telemetry sessions that were 4-10 h in duration. During each session, the radiosignal of each animal was checked for activity once each hour. An animal was considered active if (during a 20-s interval) we could hear regular variation in intensity and stability of the radiosignal, and inactive if the radiosignal had no variation as described by Riley et al. (2003). Every month we attempted to collect 2-4 independent locations/gray fox for each 1-h interval (48-96 locations/gray fox/month).

We used portable receivers (Model LA-12; AVM Instrument Company, Ltd., Colfax, California) and 4-element, hand-held, directional, Yagi antennas to locate gray foxes. We attempted to locate radiocollared foxes at least once during the day and once at night every week by traveling trails in a vehicle and hiking along ridges to get the strongest radiosignal. Locations were triangulated from 2-6 azimuths taken within 30 min by one observer, or two azimuths taken simultaneously (within 1 min) by two observers. Triangulation angles were 35-145[degrees]. Mean distance between radiocollared animals and observers during triangulation was 3596269 m. Point locations were taken >8-h apart during daytime, and >3-h apart during nighttime (when foxes were active). We estimated UTM coordinates of each location and its 95% error ellipse using the software package LOCATE II (Nams, 1990). The estimated overall standard deviation (overall error angle) for our dataset was 2.5[degrees]; average error polygons were ca. 0.1 ha.

We divided the diel period into 12 sampling intervals of 2 h each and compared activity between dry (May-October) and wet (November-April) seasons. We analyzed activity patterns by fitting a multiple-logistic-regression model as described by Hosmer and Lemeshow (1989) and Sokal and Rohlf (1994) using SPSS 9.0 statistical software. Activity status (active, inactive) was the outcome variable, and season (dry, wet) and time of day (diurnal, 0800-1559 h; crepuscular, 1600-1959 h and 0400-0759 h; nocturnal, 2000-0359 h) were independent covariates in the logistic-regression model.

Using the Geographic Information System (GIS) database of the Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area, we estimated the 100% minimum convex polygon that included the 1,244 locations of gray foxes (capture, telemetry, and mortality) and delineated a 500-m perimeter around this 100% minimum convex polygon using methods described by Hayne (1949) and Dixon and Chapman (1980). We defined this polygon (100% minimum convex polygon + 500-m perimeter) to assess use of habitats and we refer to it as the available-habitat polygon. using the GIS database and software (ArcView 3.1; Environmental Systems Research Institute, 1998), we generated 1,000 random points (without replacement) within the available-habitat polygon to estimate proportions of habitats that were available. We assessed habitat for every telemetric location and we calculated percentages for availability and use of habitats (Table 1).

We employed compositional analysis (Aebischer and Robertson, 1992; Aebischer et al., 1993) to test the null hypothesis of random habitat used by gray foxes, or to detect if selection for habitats existed. We used log-ratios of available habitats (1,000 random points, [y.sub.o]) and compositions of habitats that were used (telemetric locations, y). We excluded development areas from this analysis because no gray fox was located in developed areas. We used telemetric locations from 15 gray foxes from which we obtained >31 (range, 32-185) locations; from the other 3 foxes, we obtained < 7 locations and excluded them from analysis of composition. We replaced zero proportions (0% use) with 0.001%, which was an order of magnitude less than existing nonzero values in either available or used compositions following the method of Aebischer et al. (1993). Zero proportions were only recorded for valley oak. We calculated the difference d = y - [y.sub.o] and solved the significance of the matrix of d-values with Wilk's lambda ([lambda]) transformed as--Nln[lambda] (were N is the number of individuals), which was compared with [chi square]. We ranked the preferred habitats and tested statistical significance with the t-distribution.

Simultaneous telemetric studies of coyotes and bobcats in the Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area (Riley et al., 2003) provided the subset of activity data and locations that we used for comparison with our data for gray foxes. Coyotes and bobcats were captured, handled, and monitored similar to gray foxes (Riley et al., 2003). For comparison with gray foxes, we used activity data from the entire population of radiocollared bobcats and coyotes that were monitored during May 1996-April 1999. We obtained locations for coyotes and bobcats that were within the available-habitat polygon during May 1996-April 1999 and we calculated percentages of use for each habitat.

RESULTS--We radiocollared 24 gray foxes (12 adult males, 5 adult females, 3 subadult males, and 4 subadult females), we obtained locations on 18 of them, and we monitored 14 of them for patterns of activity. Our sample included 5,768 transmitter-days for all gray foxes (mean [+ or -] SD = 242 [+ or -] 221, range = 14-688). For analysis of use of habitat, we collected a total of 1,244 locations, including 41 capture and recapture locations, 1,191 telemetric locations, and 12 locations where dead radiocollared gray foxes were recovered. We collected 3,678 checks for activity: 1,641 checks on 8 gray foxes during the dry season and 2,037 checks on 13 gray foxes for the wet season. Activities of 69 coyotes and 45 bobcats determined by radiotelemetry were used for comparisons. We also used 195 telemetric locations of 11 bobcats and 116 of 18 coyotes within our study area for comparisons in use of habitats.

The fitted multiple-logistic-regression model that best described patterns of activity contained both time of day and season, and their interaction was significant (G = 899,df = 5, P < 0.001; H-L = 10.6, df = 8, P = 0.22). No difference in activity patterns between males and females or between adults and subadults was detected. Gray foxes were, in general, more active during nocturnal and crepuscular times than during the diurnal time interval (odds ratio = 22 and 4, respectively; Fig. 1). Overall, daily levels of activity were 60% in the summer dry season and 56% in the winter wet season. Daily levels of activity were 64 and 60% in summer for bobcats and coyotes, respectively, and 59 and 48% in winter.

Telemetric locations of gray foxes, coyotes, and bobcats differed from availability of habitats in several ways. Gray foxes used all habitats except development, coyotes were located in all habitats except valley oak, and bobcats were in all habitats (Table 1). Gray foxes preferred northern mixed chaparral over other habitats, followed by coastal sage scrub, coast live oak, then chamise chaparral; valley oak and grassland were underused (Table 2; Wilk's [lambda] = 0.208, [[chi].sup.2.sub.[5,11]] = 23.54, P < 0.001). Coyotes used coastal sage scrub significantly more than the remaining habitats, and significantly less for northern mixed chaparral. Bobcats used coast live oak significantly more than coastal sage scrub, valley oak, and grassland, and they selected against development, chamise chaparral, and northern mixed chaparral.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

DISCUSSION--In previous studies, gray foxes generally were more active at night (77-87% of telemetric checks for activity) than during the day (25-54%; Yearsley and Samuel, 1980; Haroldson and Fritzell, 1984). Activity patterns of gray foxes in our study area were similar; they may reduce activity at daytime and increase activity at nighttime during the dry season (when compared to the wet season) as strategy to avoid heat stress, or to follow activity patterns of prey. However, gray foxes may also temporally avoid larger predators; gray foxes had significantly greater probabilities of being active during nighttime and inactive during daytime than sympatric radiomonitored coyotes and bobcats, probably to reduce predatory pressures (Farias, 2000). The closely related island fox (Urocyon littoralis) is relatively active during the day but inhabits islands where no other large mammalian carnivore resides (Moore and Collins, 1995).

Gray foxes may have preferred northern mixed chaparral because their common small-mammal prey were species typical of brushy habitats (e.g., dusky-footed woodrat Neotoma lepida; Fedriani et al., 2000). However, it also was the least used habitat by sympatric coyotes and bobcats that were radiomonitored (Table 2). Gray foxes have been reported to live in developed landscapes and open habitats (Fuller, 1978; Fritzell and Haroldson, 1982; Fritzell, 1987; Harrison, 1997), butin our study, gray foxes did not use development and selected against open habitats. Coyotes and bobcats may be limiting occurrence of gray foxes in open habitats (i.e., grassland and valley oak) and developed areas in southern California (Soule et al., 1988; Crooks and Soule, 1999; Fedriani et al., 2000). Thus, there may not be enough vegetative cover for escape or protection, leaving gray foxes more vulnerable to agonistic encounters. In central Mississippi, Lovell (1996) also reported spatial segregation among coyotes, bobcats, and gray foxes. Coyotes and bobcats avoided mature stands of pines that supported a lower density of prey, whereas gray foxes preferred mature stands of pines, probably because numbers of predators were lower, and because trees were available for escape from attacks. Gray foxes seemed absent in regions with large populations of coyotes, but apparently reached their greatest abundance in regions where coyotes were scarce (Fedriani et al., 2000; Riley et al., 2003). Interestingly, most predator-killed gray foxes were killed outside or on the periphery of their ranges (Farias et al., 2005). This phenomenon also has been documented for swift foxes (Vulpes velox)by Sovada et al. (1998) and Kitchen et al. (1999) whose observations were that predation on swift foxes usually occurred away from dens and core activity areas. These authors suggested that swift foxes are more vulnerable to predation by coyotes in peripheral areas of their home range.

It is common for sympatric canids to reduce exploitative and interference competition by exhibiting both spatial and temporal segregation (Johnson et al., 1996). White et al. (1995) discovered that kit foxes (Vulpes macrotis) and coyotes exhibited habitat partitioning, but White et al. (1994) did not detect evidence of temporal segregation between these canids. Kitchen et al. (1999) reported no evidence of spatial-temporal avoidance of coyotes in movement patterns of swift foxes. Kit and swift foxes use multiple dens as a common escape route to deter attacks by coyotes (White et al., 1994, 1995; Koopman et al., 1998; Sovada et al., 1998; Kitchen et al., 1999), while gray foxes use trees to escape from predators (Wooding, 1984; Cypher, 1993). However, trees were scarce in our study area and gray foxes may find more protection under dense vegetation. Our results suggest that gray foxes in southern California may be more vulnerable to interference competition than kit or swift foxes.

Fedriani et al. (2000) suggested that gray foxes in southern California may be avoiding places and times with high risks of predation to coexist with coyotes and bobcats (Chamberlain and Leopold, 2005). Our results support this notion that spatial and temporal use of habitats by gray foxes in southern California may be regulated mainly by interference competition with other predators. Radiocollared gray foxes probably were mainly nocturnal and crepuscular to reduce predatory pressures during the day, and probably preferred northern mixed chaparral because dense vegetation provided cover for escape and this habitat had fewer predators.

This study was funded and supported by the National Park Service (Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area, California), the Department of Environmental Conservation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and the Department of Biology, University of California, Los Angeles. V. Farias was supported by Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia, Mexico, and the Fulbright Program of the Institute of International Education, USA. We gratefully acknowledge field and technical assistance of E. York, J. M. Fedriani, D. Kamradt, M. Morais, L. Lee, M. Malone, S. Ng, S. Kim, D. Jones, S. Lupus, G. Haught, and G. Busteed. T. Hosmer and E. Goldman provided statistical advice on software and logistic regression, and C. Griffin, J. Organ, and two anonymous reviewers provided critical reviews of a preliminary manuscript.

LITERATURE CITED

AEBISCHER, N. J., AND P. A. ROBERTSON. 1992. Practical aspects of compositional analysis as applied to pheasant habitat utilization. Pages 285-293 in Wildlife telemetry: remote monitoring and tracking of animals (I. G. Priede and S. M. Priede, editors). Ellis Horwood Series in Environmental Management, Science and Technology, New York.

AEBISCHER, N. J., P. A. ROBERTSON, AND R. E. KENWARD. 1993. Compositional analysis of habitat use from animal radio-tracking data. Ecology 74:1313-1325.

ANDERSON, T. M., J. G. C. HOPCRAFT, S. EBY, M. RITCHIE, J. B. GRACE, AND H. OLFF. 2010. Landscape-scale analyses suggest both nutrient and antipredator advantages to Serengeti herbivore hotspots. Ecology 91:1519-1529.

CHAMBERLAIN, M. J., AND B. D. LEOPOLD. 2005. Overlap in space use among bobcats (Lynx rufus), coyotes (Canis latrans), and gray foxes (Urocyon cinereoargenteus). American Midland Naturalist 153:171-179.

CROOKS, K. R., AND M. E. SOULE. 1999. Mesopredator release and avifaunal extinctions in a fragmented system. Nature 400:563-566.

CYPHER, B. L. 1993. Food item use by three sympatric canids in southern Illinois. Transactions of the Illinois State Academy of Science 86:139-144.

CYPHER, B. L. 2003. Foxes. Pages 511-546 in Wild mammals of North America: biology, management and conservation (G. A. Feldhamer, B. C. Thompson, and J. A. Chapman, editors). Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Maryland.

DIXON, K R., AND J. A. CHAPMAN. 1980. Harmonic mean measure of animal activity areas. Ecology 61:1040-1044.

ENVIRONMENTAL SYSTEMS RESEARCH INSTITUTE. 1998. ArcView geographic information system. Version 3.1. Environmental Systems Research Institute, Redlands, California.

FARIAS, V. 2000. Gray fox distribution in southern California: detecting the effects of intraguild predation. M.S. thesis, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

FARIAS, V., T. K. FULLER,R. K.WAYNE, AND R. M. SAUVAJOT. 2005. Survival and cause-specific mortality of gray foxes (Urocyon cinereoargenteus) in southern California. Journal of Zoology (London) 266:249-254.

FEDRIANI, J. M., T. K. FULLER, R. M. SAUVAJOT, AND E. YORK. 2000. Competition and intraguild predation among three sympatric carnivores. Oecologia (Berlin) 125:258-270.

FINKE, D. L., AND R. F. DENNO. 2002. Intraguild predation diminished in complex-structured vegetation: implications for prey suppression. Ecology 83:643-652.

FRITZELL, E. K. 1987. Gray fox and island fox. Pages 408-420 in Wild furbearer management and conservation in North America (M. Novak, J. A. Baker, M. E. Obbard, and B. Malloch, editors). Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

FRITZELL, E. K., AND K. J. HAROLDSON. 1982. Urocyon cinereoargenteus. Mammalian Species 189:1-8.

FULLER, T. K. 1978. Variable home-range sizes of female gray foxes. Journal of Mammalogy 59:446-449.

FULLER, T. K., AND B. CYPHER. 2004. Gray fox (Urocyon cinereoargenteus). Pages 92-97 in Canids: foxes, wolves, jackals, and dogs. Status survey and conservation action plan (C. Sillero-Zubiri, M. Hoffmann, and D. W. Macdonald, editors). IUCN/ Species Survival Commission, Canid Specialist Group, Gland, Switzerland.

GOSSELINK, T. E., T. R. VAN DEELEN, R. E. WARNER, AND M. G. JOSELYN. 2003. Temporal habitat partitioning and spatial use of coyotes and red foxes in east-central Illinois. Journal of Wildlife Management 67:90-103.

HAROLDSON, K. J., AND E. K. FRITZELL. 1984. Home ranges, activity, and habitat use by gray foxes in an oak-hickory forest. Journal of Wildlife Management 48:222-227.

HARRISON, R. L. 1997. A comparison of gray fox ecology between residential and undeveloped rural landscapes. Journal of Wildlife Management 61:112-122.

HAYNE, D. W. 1949. Calculation of size of home range. Journal of Mammalogy 30:1-17.

HEBBLEWHITE, M., AND E. H. MERRILL. 2009. Trade-offs between predation risk and forage differ between migrant strategies in a migratory ungulate. Ecology 90:3445-3454.

HEITHAUS, M. R. 2001. Habitat selection by predators and prey in communities with asymmetrical intraguild predation. Oikos 92:542-554.

HOSMER,D. W., AND S. LEMESHOW. 1989. Applied logistic regression. John Wiley and Sons, New York.

JOHNSON, W. E., T. K. FULLER, AND W. L. FRANKLIN. 1996. Symparty in canids: a review and assessment. Pages 198-218 in Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution (J. L. Gittleman, editor). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York.

KITCHEN, A. M., E. M. GESE, AND E. R. SCHAUSTER. 1999. Resource partitioning between coyotes and swift foxes: space, time, and diet. Canadian Journal of Zoology 77:1645-1656.

KOOPMAN, M. E., J. H. SCRIVNER, AND T. T. KATO. 1998. Patterns of den use by San Joaquin kit foxes. Journal of Wildlife Management 62:373-379.

LOVELL, C. D. 1996. Bobcat, coyote, and gray fox micro-habitat use and interspecies relationships in a managed forest in central Mississippi. M.S. thesis, Mississippi State University, State College.

MOORE,C.M., AND P. W. COLLINS. 1995. Urocyon littoralis. Mammalian Species 489:1-7.

NAMS, V. O. 1990. Locate II user's guide. Pacer Computer Software, Truro, Nova Scotia, Canada.

NATIONAL OCEANIC AND ATMOSPHERIC ADMINISTRATION. 1999. Meteorological climate summary. Years 1998 and 1999. National Weather Service, Oxnard, California.

PALOMARES,F., AND T. M. CARO. 1999. Interspecific killing among mammalian carnivores. American Naturalist 153:492-508.

POLIS, G. A., AND R. D. HOLT. 1992. Intraguild predation: the dynamics of complex trophic interactions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 7:151-154.

POLIS, G. A., C. A. MYERS, AND R. D. HOLT. 1989. The ecology and evolution of intrguild predation: potential competitors that eat each other. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 20:297-330.

RILEY, S. P. D., R. M. SAUVAJOT, T. K. FULLER, E. C. YORK, D. A. KAMRADT, C. BROMLEY, AND R. K. WAYNE. 2003. Effects of urbanization and habitat fragmentation on bobcats and coyotes in southern California. Conservation Biology 17:566-576.

ROSENHEIM, J. A. 2004. Top predators constrain the habitat selection games played by intermediate predators and their prey. Israel Journal of Zoology 50:129-138.

SANSOM, A., J. LIND, AND W. CRESSWELL. 2009. Individual behavior and survival: the roles of predator avoidance, foraging success, and vigilance. Behavioral Ecology 20:1168-1174.

SEAL, U. S., AND T. J. KREEGER. 1987. Chemical immobilization of furbearers. Pages 191-215 in Wild furbearer management and conservation in North America (M. Novak, J. A. Baker, M. E. Obbard, and B. Malloch, editors). Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

SOKAL, R. R., AND F. J. ROHLF. 1994. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research. Third edition. W. H. Freeman and Company, New York.

SOULE, M. E., D. T. BOLGER, A. C. ALBERTS, J. WRIGHT, M. SORICE, AND S. HILLS. 1988. Reconstructed dynamics of rapid extinctions of chaparral-requiring birds in urban habitat islands. Conservation Biology 2:75-95.

SOVADA, M. A., C. C. ROY, J. B. BRIGHT, AND J. R. GILLIS. 1998. Causes and rates of mortality of swift foxes in western Kansas. Journal of Wildlife Management 62:1300-1306.

SWITALSKI, T. A. 2003. Coyote foraging ecology and vigilance in response to gray wolf reintroduction in Yellowstone National Park. Canadian Journal of Zoology 81:985-993.

VALEIX, M., A. J. LOVERIDGE, S. CHAMAILLE-JAMMES, Z. DAVIDSON, F. MURINDAGOMO, H. FRITZ, AND D. W. MACDONALD. 2009. Behavioral adjustments of African herbivores to predation risk by lions: spatiotemporal variations influence habitat use. Ecology 90:23-30.

WHITE, P. J., K. RALLS, AND R. A. GARROT. 1994. Coyote-kit fox interactions as revealed by telemetry. Canadian Journal of Zoology 72:1831-1836.

WHITE, P. J., K. RALLS, AND C. A. VANDERBILT-WHITE. 1995. Overlap in food and habitat use between coyotes and San Joaquin kit foxes. Southwestern Naturalist 40:342-349.

WOODING, J. B. 1984. Coyote food habits and the spatial relationship of coyotes and foxes in Mississippi and Alabama. M.S. thesis, Mississippi State University, State College.

YEARSLEY, E. F., AND D. E. SAMUEL. 1980. Use of reclaimed surface mines by foxes in West Virginia. Journal of Wildlife Management 44:729-734.

Submitted 19 October 2010.

Accepted 21 February 2012.

Associate Editor was Floyd W. Weckerly.

VERONICA FARIAS, TODD K. FULLER, * AND RAYMOND M. SAUVAJOT

Department of Environmental Conservation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003 (VF, TKF)

Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area, National Park Service, 401 Hillcrest Drive, Thousand Oaks, CA 91360 (RMS)

Present address of VF: Laboratorio de Recursos Naturales, Unidad de Biologia, Tecnologia y Prototipos, Facultad de Estudios

Superiores, Campus Iztacala, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, Avenida de los Barrios 1, Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, C. P. 54090, Estado de Mexico, Mexico

* Correspondent: tkfuller@eco.umass.edu
TABLE 1--Percentages (6 SEE) of habitats available and used (range
in parentheses) by three sympatric carnivores (gray fox, Urocyon
cinereoargenteus; coyote, Canis latrans; and bobcat, Lynx rufus) in
the Santa Monica Mountains, Ventura and Los Angeles counties,
California, during May 1996-July 1999.

Habitat          Habitats    Habitats used by each species
                 available
                                        Gray fox

Coastal sage        40           29 [+ or -] 13 (0-48)
  scrub
Northern mixed      34          51 [+ or -] 16 (30-100)
  chaparral
Development         11                     0
Chamise              6            9 [+ or -] 9 (0-27)
  chaparral
Coast live oak       5           10 [+ or -] 11 (0-38)
Grassland            3            <1 [+ or -] <1 (0-6)
Valley oak          <1            <1 [+ or -] <1 (0-9)

Habitat                   Habitats used by each species

                         Coyote                   Bobcat

Coastal sage     58 [+ or -] 38 (0-100)   47 [+ or -] 32 (0-100)
  scrub
Northern mixed    7 [+ or -] 14 (0-50)     4 [+ or -] 8 (0-25)
  chaparral
Development      12 [+ or -] 27 (0-100)    9 [+ or -] 21 (0-70)
Chamise           2 [+ or -] 5 (0-20)       1 [+ or -] 2 (0-7)
  chaparral
Coast live oak   14 [+ or -] 33 (0-100)   28 [+ or -] 7 (0-100)
Grassland         9 [+ or -] 20 (0-67)     8 [+ or -] 12 (0-36)
Valley oak                 0               4 [+ or -] 7 (0-25)

TABLE 2--Compositional analysis of relative use of habitats by
mesocarnivores (gray fox Urocyon cinereoargenteus, coyote Canis
latrans, and bobcat Lynx rufus) in the Santa Monica Mountains,
Ventura and Los Angeles counties, California, during May 1996-July
1999

Species    n    order of compositional preference (a)

Gray fox   18   northern mixed chaparral >> coastal sage scrub >
                coast live oak >> chamise chaparral >> valley
                oak > grassland >> development

Coyote     18   coastal sage scrub >> valley oak > grassland >
                coast live oak >> development > chamise
                chaparral > northern mixed chaparral

Bobcat     11   coast live oak >> coastal sage scrub > valley
                oak > grassland >> development > chamise
                chaparral > northern mixed chaparral

(a) >> Indicates significant difference between habitats.
COPYRIGHT 2012 Southwestern Association of Naturalists
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2012 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Farias, Veronica; Fuller, Todd K.; Sauvajot, Raymond M.
Publication:Southwestern Naturalist
Article Type:Report
Geographic Code:1USA
Date:Jun 1, 2012
Words:4662
Previous Article:Reproductive ecology and characteristics of denning sites of the San Clemente Island fox.
Next Article:Genetic structure of a population of the endangered star cactus (Astrophytum asterias) in southern Texas.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2018 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters