Printer Friendly

Monitoring memories moving in the brain.

You're trying to find your car in a crowded parking lot. You remember what you car looks like, the appearance of the lamppost you parked beside, and glancing at that lamppost over your shoulder as you walked off the parking lot. How can you piece these bits of information together to find your car?

If you'd parked just a few hours before - to go shopping, for example - a new experiment indicates that you would use a banana-shaped region of your brain called the hippocampus to navigate your way back to your car. But if you'd parked weeks before - says, at an airport before embarking on a month-long trip -- the study suggests that you would have to retrieve the information about your car's whereabouts from another part of your brain, probably the neocortex, where longer-term memories are stored.

In a series of tests involving rats, behavioral neuroscientists Michael S. Fanselow and Jeansok J. Kim of the University of California, Los Angeles, have demonstrated what physicians treating brain-damaged patients have suspected for nearly 100 years -- that the hippocampus plays a vital role in the short-term memory of "contextual" information, such as the series of clues you would recall to find your car in a crowded lot. The researchers report their finding in the May 1 Science.

Fanselow and Kim trained 22 rats to associate being in a specific type of box while hearing a given tone with receiving a small electrical shock to their feet. The box was square, had an even floor and smelled of ammonica.

The researchers then damaged the hippocampi of eight of the rats at four different times after the training: one day, one week, two weeks and four weeks. For controls, Fanselow and Kim damaged the brain region just above the hippocampus in a group of eight rats day after training and left the brains of another group of six rats undamaged.

To test whether hippocampal damage affected the rats' ability to remember the shock, the researchers put each of the animals back into the box. While both groups of control animals showed their fear of the box by crouchiing in a corner and "freezing," Fanselow and Kim observed that the rats whose hippocampi had been damaged the day after training ran around the box normally, as if they had forgotten their shock experience. Rats whose hippocampi had been damaged four weeks after training were as affraid of the box as both groups of controls.

When Fanselow and Kim put each of the animals into a triangular box with an uneven floor that smelled of vinegar, they found that none of the animals showed fear of their surroundings. But when they turned on the one, all of the rats -- including those with damaged hippocampi -- became afraid.

"The animals which didn't have hippocampal damage showed a fear of the tone and a fear of the place where they got a shock," summarizes Fanselow. "But while some of the animals that had a damaged hippocampus showed no fear of the plae, they were afraid of the tone."

He concludes that the hippocampus is involved in learning that requires integrating various stimuli -- such as the look, feel and smell of a box or the different views of a car in a parking lot -- but not in learning a single stimulus, such as the tone. Moreover, he says, the study shows that the hippocampus is necessary only for the short-term recall of such memories. After a certain period, Fanselow theorizes, the memories are transferred to another part of the brain, such as the neocortex, for long-term storage.

"This experiment demonstrates that the contribution of the hippocampus is only temporary," says Larry R. Squire, a neuroscientist at the San Diego Veterans Administration Medical Center. But he cautions that "memories may well be in the neocortex all along, with the hippocampus just playing a role in storage and retrieval. . . . Whether memories move or not is not so clear, but that's certainly one possibility."
COPYRIGHT 1992 Science Service, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 1992, Gale Group. All rights reserved. Gale Group is a Thomson Corporation Company.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Ezzell, Carol
Publication:Science News
Date:May 2, 1992
Words:660
Previous Article:California faults unleash week of turmoil.
Next Article:Radwastes may escape glass via new route.
Topics:


Related Articles
Watching the remembering brain at work.
For a good memory, dream on.
Conscious memories may emerge in infants.
Brain region linked to conscious memories.
Do old brain cells die?
Motor cortex helps drive serial memory.
Thinking About THINKING: Harnessing the Brain's POWER.
Certain memories may rest on a good sleep.
A tip of the tongue to the brain.
Brain area may support fact and event memory. (Neural Recall).

Terms of use | Copyright © 2016 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters