Printer Friendly
The Free Library
22,728,960 articles and books

Emblems of awareness: brain signatures lead scientists to the seat of consciousness.



[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

Humankind's sharpest minds have figured out some of nature's deepest secrets. Why the sun shines. How humans evolved from single-celled life. Why an apple falls to the ground. Humans have conceived and built giant telescopes that glimpse galaxies billions of light-years away and microscopes that illuminate the contours of a single atom. Yet the peculiar quality that enabled such flashes of scientific insight and grand achievements remains a mystery: consciousness.

Though in some ways deeply familiar, consciousness is at the same time foreign to those in its possession. Deciphering the cryptic machinations of the brain--and how they create a mind--poses one of the last great challenges facing the scientific world.

For a long time, the very question was considered to be in poor taste, acceptable for philosophical musing but outside the bounds of real science. Whispers of the C-word were met with scorn in polite scientific society.

Toward the end of the last century, though, sentiment shifted as some respectable scientists began saying the C-word out loud. Initially these discussions were tantalizing tan·ta·lize  
tr.v. tan·ta·lized, tan·ta·liz·ing, tan·ta·liz·es
To excite (another) by exposing something desirable while keeping it out of reach.
 but hazy: Like kids parroting a dirty word without knowing what it means, scientists speculated on what consciousness is without any real data. After a while, though, researchers developed ways to turn their instruments inward to study the very thing that was doing the studying.

Today consciousness research has become a passion for many scientists, and not just for the thrill of saying a naughty word. A flood of data is sweeping brain scientists far beyond their intuitions, for the first time enabling meaningful evidence-based discussions about the nature of consciousness.

"You're not condemned to walk around in this epistemological fog where it's all just sort of philosophy and speculation," says neuroscientist Christof Koch of Caltech and the Allen Institute for Brain Science in Seattle. "It used to be the case, but now we can attack this question experimentally, using the tools of good old science to try to come to grips with it."

Knowledge emerging from all of this work has ushered researchers into a rich cycle of progress. New experimental results have guided theoretical concepts of consciousness, which themselves churn out predictions that can be tested with more refined experiments. Ultimately, these new insights could answer questions such as whether animals, or the Internet, or the next-generation iPhone could ever possess consciousness.

Though a detailed definition remains elusive, in simplest terms, consciousness is what you lose when you fall into a deep sleep at night and what you gain when you wake up in the morning. A brain that is fully awake and constructing experiences is said to be fully conscious. By comparing such brains with others that are in altered states of awareness, researchers are identifying some of the key ingredients that a conscious brain requires.

In the hunt for these ingredients, "we decided to go for big changes in consciousness," says Giulio Tononi of the University of Wisconsin-Madison “University of Wisconsin” redirects here. For other uses, see University of Wisconsin (disambiguation).
A public, land-grant institution, UW-Madison offers a wide spectrum of liberal arts studies, professional programs, and student activities.
. He and others are studying brains that are deeply asleep, under anesthesia or even in comas, searching for dimmer switches that dial global levels of consciousness up or down.

Scrutinizing brain changes that correspond to such levels has led some scientists to a central hub deep in the brain. Called the thalamus thalamus (thăl`əməs), mass of nerve cells centrally located in the brain just below the cerebrum and resembling a large egg in size and shape. , this structure is responsible for constantly sending and receiving a torrent of neural missives. Other clues to consciousness come from a particular kind of electrical signal that the brain produces when it becomes aware of something in the outside world. But rather than one kind of signature, or one strategic brain structure, consciousness depends on many regions and signals working in concert. The key maybe in the exquisitely complicated ebb and flow the alternate ebb and flood of the tide; often used figuratively.

See also: Ebb
 of the brain's trillions of connections.

Hub of activity

A profoundly damaged thalamus turned out to be at the center of one of the first right-to-die battles in the United States. A heart attack in 1975 left 21-year-old Karen Ann Quinlan Karen Ann Quinlan (March 29 1954 – June 11 1985) was an important figure in the history of the right to die debate in United States. When she was 21, Quinlan fell unconscious after coming home from a party, and lapsed into a persistent vegetative state.  in a nonresponsive, unconscious vegetative state Vegetative State Definition

A coma-like state characterized by open eyes and the appearance of wakefulness is defined as vegetative.
Description

The vegetative state is a chronic or long-term condition.
 for a decade. After she ultimately died of natural causes, an autopsy revealed surprising news: Quinlan's cerebral cortex cerebral cortex

Layer of gray matter that constitutes the outer layer of the cerebrum and is responsible for integrating sensory impulses and for higher intellectual functions.
, the outer layer of the brain where thoughts are formed, appeared relatively unscathed. But the thalamus was destroyed.

The thalamus is made up of two robin's egg--sized structures that perch atop the brain stem, a perfect position to serve as the brain's busiest busybody. It is the first stop for many of the stimuli that come into the brain from the eyes, ears, tongue and skin. Like a switchboard operator, after gathering information from particular senses, the thalamus shoots the signals along specific nerve fibers, connecting the right signal to the right part of the brain's wrinkly cortex.

These strong connections, along with evidence from vegetative state patients, make the thalamus a prime suspect in the hunt for the seat of consciousness. A 2010 study in the Journal of Neurotrauma, for example, found atrophy of the thalamus in people in a vegetative state.

Not only is the thalamus itself compromised, but also its connections--white-matter tracts that carry nerve signals--seem to be dysfunctional in people who aren't fully conscious, researchers reported last year in NeuroImage.

"I can't help but think there's something fundamental about the functional circuitry," says neuroscientist David Edelman of the Neurosciences Institute in San Diego. "There's a fundamental loop between ... the thalamus and the cortex. If those connections are cut or if you've damaged them, that individual will not be aware by any measure, forever."

One of the most startling pieces of evidence implicating the thalamus came from a patient who had existed in a minimally conscious state A minimally conscious state (MCS) is a condition distinct from coma or the vegetative state, in which a patient exhibits deliberate, or cognitively mediated, behavior often enough, or consistently enough, for clinicians to be able to distinguish it from entirely unconscious,  for six years, drifting in and out of awareness. After surgery in which doctors implanted electrodes that stimulated his thalamus, the man began responding more consistently to commands, moved his muscles and even spoke.

But the part the thalamus plays in consciousness is not straightforward. Its role may be as complex as the intricate spidery connections linking it to the rest of the brain.

"The thalamus has two souls," says Martin Monti, a neuroscientist at the University of California, Los Angeles UCLA comprises the College of Letters and Science (the primary undergraduate college), seven professional schools, and five professional Health Science schools. Since 2001, UCLA has enrolled over 33,000 total students, and that number is steadily rising. . One of the souls receives information directly from the outside world, and one receives information from other parts of the brain. "It turns out that there are many more connections going from cortex back to thalamus," he says. "There's a lot of chitchat."

This huge influx of messages from the cortex may mean that the thalamus is simply a very sensitive readout of cortical behavior, as work reported in 2007 in Anesthesiology hints.

As anesthesia took hold of participants in the study, activity in the cortex wavered, yet the thalamus kept chugging away normally for about 10 minutes. If the thalamus were the ultimate arbiter of consciousness, its behavior should have changed before that of the cortex.

Instead of being a driver, the thalamus may be a consciousness gauge. In the same way that a thermometer can tell you to grab a coat but doesn't actually make it cold, the thalamus may tell you a person is conscious without making it so.

Reading waves

Rather than studying the thalamus, some researchers focus on long-range brain waves brain waves Neurology Oscillations/sec that correspond to various types of cerebral activity, as measured on an EEG. See Electroencephalogram.  that ripple over the cortex. One such ripple, a fast electrical signal called a gamma wave, has garnered a lot of attention. These waves, which in some cases emanate from the thalamus, are generated by the combined electrical activity of coalitions of nerve cells behaving similarly. Gamma waves spread over the brain at about 40 waves per second; other brain waves--such as those thought to mark extreme concentration or attention--are slower.

Gamma waves have been spotted along with mental processes such as memory, attention, hearing noises and seeing objects. And studies have even found that the waves are present in REM [Latin, In the thing itself.] A lawsuit against an item of property, not against a person (in personam).

An action in rem is a proceeding that takes no notice of the owner of the property but determines rights in the property that are conclusive against all the
 sleep, the stage marked by intense dreams.

Such associations have led some researchers to propose that gamma waves bind disparate pieces of a scene, tying together the rumble of a boat's outboard, the crisp breeze and a memory of a black lab into a unified lake experience.

But some new data call gamma waves' role in consciousness into question, by finding that the signal can be present when consciousness is not. Researchers, including Tononi, monitored electrical signals in brains of people as anesthesia took hold. When eight healthy people were anesthetized with propofol (the powerful anesthetic that Michael Jackson used to sleep), gamma waves actually increased, the team reported last year in Sleep. Consciousness was clearly diminished, yet the gamma waves persisted.

Specific brain signals, such as gamma waves, might be important aspects of consciousness, but not the main driving forces in the brain. "I can put gamma waves into any machine," says Tononi. But doing so won't give the machine a conscious mind.

The same may be true for structures such as the thalamus, as well as other regions that have been scrutinized by scientists, including the parietal parietal /pa·ri·e·tal/ (pah-ri´e-t'l)
1. of or pertaining to the walls of a cavity.

2. pertaining to or located near the parietal bone.


pa·ri·e·tal
adj.
1.
 and frontal cortices cor·ti·ces  
n.
A plural of cortex.
, the reticular activating system reticular activating system
n. Abbr. RAS
The part of the reticular formation in the brainstem that plays a central role in bodily and behavioral alertness; its ascending connections affect the function of the cerebral cortex and its
 in the brain stem and a thin sheetlike structure called the claustrum.

Increasingly nuanced views of the ingredients at work in a conscious brain have led some scientists to a new suspicion: Perhaps the thing in the brain that underlies consciousness is not a thing at all, but a process. Messages constantly zing around the brain in complex patterns, as if trillions of tiny balls were simultaneously dropped into a pinball machine, each with a prescribed, mission-critical path. This constant flow of information might be what creates consciousness--and interruptions might destroy it.

Crucial connections

One way to look for signs of interrupted information flow is by conducting brain scans as propofol takes effect. In a study published last July in NeuroImage, 18 healthy volunteers were administered the anesthetic while in a functional MRI 1. (application) MRI - Magnetic Resonance Imaging.
2. MRI - Measurement Requirements and Interface.
 brain scanner, fMRI approximates a brain region's activity by measuring blood flow: The busier the brain region, the more blood flows there.

While deeply anesthetized, some brain regions that normally operate in tandem fell out of sync, Jessica Schrouff of the University of Liege liege

In European feudal society, an unconditional bond between a man and his overlord. Thus, if a tenant held estates from various overlords, his obligations to his liege lord, to whom he had paid “liege homage,” were greater than his obligations to the other
 in Belgium and colleagues reported. Conversations within particular brain areas, and also between far-flung brain areas, fell apart.

People in vegetative vegetative /veg·e·ta·tive/ (vej?e-ta?tiv)
1. of, pertaining to, or characteristic of plants.

2. concerned with growth and nutrition, as opposed to reproduction.

3.
 states also appear to have interruptions in brain connections, Melanie Boly of the University of Liege and colleagues found after comparing these patients with healthy volunteers. Participants listened to a series of tones, most of which were similar, but every so often, a strange "oddball" tone would play, spurring a big reaction in the brain. The initial brain reaction in vegetative state patients was normal, as measured by EEG EEG: see electroencephalography.  monitors.

The signal seemed to travel from the auditory regions of the brain to other areas in the cortex. But the signal stopped there. Unlike in healthy people, the pinball-like motion of information traveling from different sites in the cortex didn't make its way back down to the auditory regions that first responded to the tone, the team reported last May in Science.

It's not clear just what causes these disconnects. One possible culprit, as counterintuitive coun·ter·in·tu·i·tive  
adj.
Contrary to what intuition or common sense would indicate: "Scientists made clear what may at first seem counterintuitive, that the capacity to be pleasant toward a fellow creature is ...
 as it seems, may be an overload of synchrony synchrony /syn·chro·ny/ (-krah-ne) the occurrence of two events simultaneously or with a fixed time interval between them.

atrioventricular (AV) synchrony
, Gernot Supp of the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf in Germany and colleagues reported in December in Current Biology. As an anesthetic kicks in, huge swaths of the brain adopt slow, uniform behavior. This hypersynchrony, as it's called, may be one way that anesthesia stamps out the back-and-forth of information in the brain.

Instead of just observing the brain's behavior and inferring connectivity, Tononi, Marcello Massimini of the University of Milan The university is a member of the League of European Research Universities.

Throughout Milan, the University is normally known as Statale to avoid confusion with other academic institutions in the city.
 in Italy and colleagues decided to manipulate the brain directly. The team figured out how to use a technique called transcranial magnetic stimulation magnetic stimulation Neurology A noninvasive method for stimulating the brain and nerves, with a high-current magnetic pulse passed through a coil of wire , or TMS TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (alternative medicine for depression)
TMS Test Match Special (sports - cricket)
TMS Texas Motor Speedway
TMS Transportation Management System
TMS Toyota Motor Sales
, to jolt a small part of the brain and monitor the resulting signals with electrodes.

"Basically you trigger a chain of reactions in the cerebral cortex," Massimini says. "It's like we're knocking on the brain with this pulse, and then we see how this knocking propagates."

Like ripples on a pond, the reverberation from the TMS in a healthy, alert person was a complex, widely spreading pattern lasting about 300 milliseconds.

This complex entity became much simpler, though, when the brain was deeply asleep. Instead of morphing from one shape to another like a drop of food coloring that roils around in water before dissipating, the signal sits right where it started, and it fades faster, disappearing after about 150 milliseconds. The same simple pattern is found in anesthetized brains.

"If you knock on a wooden table or a bucket full of nothing, you get different noises," Massimini says. "If you knock on the brain that is healthy and conscious, you get a very complex noise." Massimini, Tononi and colleagues have recently found the same stunted response in patients in a vegetative state. The team tested five vegetative state patients, five minimally conscious patients and two people who were fully conscious but unable to move (a condition called locked-in syndrome). For the most part, locked-in patients and minimally conscious patients showed complex and long-lasting signals in the brain, similar to fully conscious people. But vegetative state patients' brains showed a brief, stagnant signal, the team reported online in January in Brain.

Such clear-cut differences in the brain could one day help in diagnosing people who have some level of consciousness but are unable to interact with doctors. When researchers performed the test on five new patients who shifted to a vegetative state in the months after coming out of a coma, three of the five regained consciousness. Before the doctors saw clinical signs of improvement, the method picked up increases in brain connectivity.

At this stage, the measurement is somewhat coarse, Massimini says. But further refinements may allow doctors to better assess levels of consciousness.

Looking at these large-scale changes in the brain may also provide some new leads to scientists puzzling over what consciousness means. Other ideas will probably come from scientists studying a different facet of consciousness: how the brain builds whole experiences out of many small pieces, such as the crisp taste of an apple, the rustle rus·tle  
v. rus·tled, rus·tling, rus·tles

v.intr.
1. To move with soft fluttering or crackling sounds.

2. To move or act energetically or with speed.

3. To forage food.
 of fall leaves and a feeling of joy.

Approaching consciousness from a lot of different angles is the best bet for ultimately understanding it, says neuroscientist Anil Seth of the Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science in Brighton, England.

In the same way that "life" evades a single, clear definition (growth, reproduction or a healthy metabolism could all apply), consciousness might turn out to be a collection of remarkable phenomena, Seth says. "If we can explain different aspects of consciousness, then my hope is that it will start to seem slightly less mysterious that there is consciousness at all in the universe."

Demystifying the Mind

A three-part series on the scientific struggle to explain the conscious self.

IN THIS ISSUE

"Emblems of awareness"

A flood of brain data may yield the recipe for consciousness.

"Self as symbol," PAGE 28

A mind's attempts to understand itself generate a strange loop.

IN THE FEBRUARY 25 ISSUE

"Consciousness emerges"

Conscious experience coalesces from sensory inputs traveling through the brain.

IN THE MARCH 10 ISSUE

"Enriched with information"

Rich, deeply connected information may be the defining feature of consciousness.

Recipe for consciousness

Somehow a sense of self emerges from the many interactions of nerve cells and neurotransmitters in the brain--but a single source behind the phenomenon remains elusive.

Parietal cortex Brain activity in the parietal cortex is diminished by anesthetics, when people fall into a deep sleep and in people in a vegetative state or coma. There is some evidence suggesting that the parietal cortex is where first-person perspective is generated.

[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

Thalamus As one of the brain's busiest hubs of activity, the thalamus is believed by many to have an important role in consciousness. Damage to even a small spot in the thalamus can lead to consciousness disorders.

Reticular activating system Damage to a particular group of nerve cell clusters, called the reticular activating system and found in the brain stem, can render a person comatose co·ma·tose
adj.
1. Of, relating to, or affected with coma.

2. Marked by lethargy; torpid.


comatose (kō´m
.

Frontal cortex Some researchers argue that parts of the frontal cortex (along with connections to the parietal cortex) are required for consciousness. But other scientists point to a few studies in which people with damaged frontal areas retain consciousness.

Claustrum An enigmatic, thin sheet of neural tissue called the claustrum has connections with many other regions. Though the structure has been largely ignored by modern scientists, Francis Crick became keenly interested in the claustrum's role in consciousness just before his death in 2004.

Degrees of thought Awareness typically tracks with wakefulness--especially in normal states of consciousness (bold). People in coma or under general anesthesia score low on both measures, appearing asleep with no signs of awareness. Sometimes, wakefulness wakefulness

believed to occur when the tonic flow of impulses from the reticular activating system exceeds the critical level for sustaining consciousness; reduction of reticular activating system activity is the basis of the pharmacological induction of sedation.
 and awareness become uncoupled, such as among people in a persistent vegetative state persistent vegetative state: see under coma, in medicine. . In this case, a person seems awake and is sometimes able to move but is unaware of the surroundings.

SOURCE: STANFORD UNIV UNIV University
UNIV Universal
.

[GRAPHIC OMITTED]

Brain jolt

In a recent study, a team injected a signal (cross) into the brain via a technique called transcranial magnetic stimulation. In fully awake volunteers (brain of one shown), a long-lasting response flooded the cortex. Dreaming patients showed some reverberation, but the response was stunted during deep non-REM sleep.

[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

Explore more

* The Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science website: www.suxxes.ac.uk/sackler

* M. Rosanova et al. "Recovery of cortical effective connectivity and recovery of consciousness in vegetative patients," Brain, January 6, 2012.
COPYRIGHT 2012 Science Service, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2012 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

 Reader Opinion

Title:

Comment:



 

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Sanders, Laura
Publication:Science News
Geographic Code:1USA
Date:Feb 11, 2012
Words:2894
Previous Article:Deep life: teeming masses of organisms thrive beneath the seafloor.
Next Article:Self as symbol: the loopy nature of consciousness trips up scientists studying themselves.
Topics:

Terms of use | Copyright © 2014 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters