Printer Friendly

Does PMDD belong in the DSM? challenging the medicalization of women's bodies.

Abstract: Many believe in the existence of a class of symptoms experienced prior to menses labelled as Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD), however, the research findings on PMDD have been tenuous. In this paper, the validity and utility of the PMDD diagnosis is" called into question and it is argued that PMDD is a socially constructed diagnosis rather than a psychiatric disorder. The distinction between physiological experiences and pathology is investigated in addition to the implications of a PMDD diagnosis for patients, clinicians, researchers and pharmaceutical companies. Finally, the paper explores the relation between PMDD and sexuality offering an additional voice in the developing discourse on the medicalization of women's experiences of sexuality.

Key words: Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD) Women's bodies DSM Sexuality Social construction

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the November 2002 meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality in Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

INTRODUCTION

Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD) is identified in the current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association (DSM-IV-TR) as a severe form of premenstrual distress (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). It is presented as a diagnostic category proposed for further study because of controversy surrounding the diagnosis. While the clinical and pharmacological literature reports on symptomatology and treatment of PMDD (e.g., Freeman & Sondheimer, 2003), another body of literature questions the consistency of research on premenstrual disorders in general and the reliability of the empirical support for the PMDD diagnostic category in particular (e.g., McFarlane & Williams, 1990; Rodin, 1992; Steiner & Born, 2000). A central theme in the debate is the utility and validity of the diagnostic category. Another is the extent to which categorization of menstrual cycle-related changes as PMDD represents a continuation of the medicalization of women's experiences of their bodies (Chrisler & Caplan, 2002).

This paper explores the concept of the medicalization of women's experiences of their bodies within the context of the continuing debate over the pathologizing of menstrual cycle-related changes in general and PMDD in particular. We review the history of research on premenstrual diagnostic categories and present literature that calls into question the utility and validity of PMDD as a psychiatric disorder and suggest, instead, that it might be better viewed as a socially constructed diagnosis. The paper also explores the relation between PMDD and women's sexuality and the implications of PMDD diagnosis for women, researchers, and clinicians.

From a medical perspective, physical and psychological experiences associated with the menstrual cycle become symptoms when they appreciably affect quality of life and day-to-day functioning. Our intent in this review is not to deny that women experience such changes, which can include bloating, weight gain, breast tenderness and swelling, appetite changes and sleep disturbances (Davis & Yonkers, 1997), or that they report the more severe, and less common, experiences identified as PMDD. Rather, we examine the longstanding debate over the labelling of menstrual cycle-related changes as symptoms and the extent to which a history of such labelling has influenced the meaning attached to these experiences and our responses to them. For example, Tavris (1992) notes that,
 ... the changes associated with the menstrual
 cycle are "real," are felt physically, and that
 they provide a fuel for moods and feelings.
 But the content of those moods and wishes
 often depends on a woman's attitudes,
 expectations, situation, personal history, and
 immediate problems or concerns (p. 153).


Tavris (1992) does not suggest that all biomedical premenstrual research should be viewed as adding to this negative discourse.
 Hormone studies are part of an ongoing tidal
 wave of biological research in general, and
 much of this research has benefitted women.
 Women should know that the physiological
 changes of the menstrual cycle vary
 enormously, that normal women range from
 having no pain or discomfort to having
 considerable though temporary pain (p. 132).


However, she cautions against the "over psychologizing of normal biological processes" but also warns against the "dangers of reducing all of our feelings, problems, and conflicts to them" (p. 133).

This caution is pertinent in the context of the diagnosis of PMDD which is reported to affect 2-9% of women and to include severe symptomatology including depression (Freeman & Sondheimer, 2003). The use of serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) for treatment of depressive symptoms in women diagnosed with PMDD (for review, see Ackerman and Williams, 2002) appears to be shifting the balance of discourse even further toward the notion of a biological disorder (e.g., defects in the serotonergic system in the brain) in the face of what some consider to be insufficient evidence and without due consideration of alternative views. As Caplan (2004) states, "The problem with PMDD is not the women who report that they have premenstrual emotional problems; the problem is with the diagnosis of PMDD itself" (p. 62). In this respect, it is the diagnosis of PMDD that leads to the medicalization of women's experiences of the menstrual cycle and not the cycle-related changes themselves.

Since this paper reviews literature that shares Caplan's concerns, it is important to define what medicalization means in this context. Medicalization occurs when a social situation or personal experience is made into a medical problem that requires the attention of medical experts (Conrad & Schneider, 1980). Tiefer (2002) states that,
 The involvement of doctors, diagnostic tests,
 medical language, surgeries and drugs has
 mystified women's experiences of life events
 such as menstruation, menopause, pregnancy
 and childbirth and not improved them as much
 as an uncritical view of modern medicine
 would have us believe (p. 128).


The effect has been that women's subjective experiences of menstrual cycle-related changes have been marginalized or negated (Ussher, 2002). The debate surrounding the clinical categorization of menstrual cycle-related changes as PMDD is the most recent aspect of a continuing discourse on this issue.

A HISTORY OF RESEARCH ON PREMENSTRUAL DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES

The first mention of menstrual cycle-related changes in the medical literature is attributed to Frank (1931) who used the term "premenstrual tension" to describe a constellation of menstrual cycle-related changes experienced by women during the second half of the menstrual cycle. Dalton and Greene (1953) coined the term "premenstrual syndrome," initiating what Chrisler and Caplan (2002) characterize as a major step forward in the medicalization of the menstrual cycle. It was not until the 1980s that a disorder based on menstrual cycle-related changes appeared in the DSM. Late Luteal Phase Dysphoric Disorder (LLPDD) was included as an "Unspecified Mental Disorder" presented for further study in the DSM-III-R in 1987. LLPDD was not a replacement for PMS but a new category that was characterized by recurrent dysphoria in synchrony with the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Symptoms were similar to those of PMS but represented a more severe and debilitating form with a smaller percentage of women being covered by the category (Fink, 1987).

The LLPDD category was met with controversy (Caplan, 2004). Although some researchers felt that the category of LLPDD provided a more standard definition and would facilitate better understanding of premenstrual symptoms as a single disorder, others felt that the category might allow for misinterpretations and misuse (Rodin, 1992; Ussher, 1996). Indeed, in response to pressure from women's groups (i.e., Women in Psychology, the Association of Women Psychiatrists, APA Division 35 and, the Feminist Therapy Institute), the American Psychological Association, opposed the inclusion of the LLPDD category in the DSM-III-R (Ussher, 1996). As a result, the LLPDD category was placed in the appendix "for categories requiring further study" (Caplan, 2004). This background to LLPDD reflects a strong tradition of questioning premenstrual disorders (Caplan, McGurdy & Gans, 1992; Chrisler & Caplan, 2002; Endicott, 2000; Figert, 1996; Severino, 1996). The imminent release of the DSM-V is likely to see continuation of the debate over premenstrual diagnostic categories.

LLPDD remained in the DSM until it was replaced by PMDD in the DSM-IV, also as a category for further study, in 1994. Although the name for the diagnostic category changed from LLPDD to PMDD in the DSM-IV, there was little difference between the two categories. The minor changes to the category included: changing sections, reordering the symptoms and adding a new symptom (a subjective sense of being overwhelmed or out of control). Although some controversy existed over the classification of PMDD as a mental disorder, as with LLPDD in the DSM-III-R, the main debate over PMDD focused on the symptoms and how these symptoms would determine location of PMDD in the manual.

Research revealed that the most frequently reported symptom of LLPDD was depression; therefore, work group members felt it should top the list of symptoms for diagnosing PMDD (Severino, 1996). This emphasis on depression created controversy within work group members when determining PMDD's placement in the DSM. Some of the work group members felt that it should remain in the same section as was LLPDD in the DSM-R-III, while others felt it should be placed in the section of "Depressive Disorders not otherwise specified" (Frank & Severino, 1995), thereby linking menstruation with depression. In the end, the category of PMDD was placed in the Appendix B "Criteria and Axes Provided for Further Study."

THE CHALLENGES IN PREMENSTRUAL-RELATED RESEARCH

While the categories of LLPDD and PMDD have been identified as advances in the provision of "operationally defined diagnostic criteria for PMS" (Freeman & Sondheimer, 2003, p. 30), the history of research in this area reflects difficulty with categorization and with precise identification of definitive symptoms for different categories (McFarlane & Williams, 1990). This section cites research published before the LLPDD category appeared, after LLPDD but before PMDD, and post-PMDD. Distinctions will be drawn accordingly although the continuity of earlier challenges surrounding PMS with current debates about PMDD will be apparent. Premenstrual-related research has been characterized by an inability to find significant biological differences in women who experience premenstrual symptoms that distinguish them from women who do not. Richardson (1995) points out that no consistent endocrinological pattern has been established for women presenting with PMS that can differentiate them. Alberts and Alberts (1990) made a similar claim in relation to possible biological factors associated with PMS.

Researchers seeking psychological explanations for PMS have had similar difficulty in distinguishing between women with and without symptoms (Rubinow & Roy-Byrne, 1984). Olasov & Jackson (1987) reported a general inability to find cyclical changes in affect associated with the menstrual cycle and Schnurr, Hurt and Stout (1994) found no significant psychological differences between women with and without LLPDD. Much of the psychological research done on women with premenstrual symptoms is correlational, thereby making it impossible to determine whether or not the psychological symptoms are causes or consequences of the menstrual cycle-related changes. However, causality has often been assumed when only a correlation exists (Caplan et al., 1992). Although significant correlational findings exist between some psychological disorders and premenstrual disorders, a number of researchers have pointed out the methodological limitations of such studies (Caplan et al., 1992; Pearlstein, Frank, Rivera-Yovar, Thoft, Jacobs, & Mieczkowski, 1990). Although some findings suggest that women who experience PMS may have higher rates of affective disorders, these studies do not help to clearly differentiate women who experience PMS from those who do not (Ussher, 1996). Depression is a significant symptom in the current PMDD diagnostic category, but here too the mixed benefits of antidepressant treatment raise questions. In their review of treatment options, Ackermann & Williams (2002) conclude: "There are considerable areas of uncertainty regarding all aspects of minor depression and PMDD. The most pressing needs are for better studies of the natural history and validated clinical predictors of poor outcome" (p. 299).

PMDD AND THE DSM PROCESS FOR DEFINING PSYCHIATRIC DISORDERS

In order for any mental disorder to be included in the DSM it must go through a process of definition that includes: peer reviews, conferences and workshops, semiannual newsletters, and communication with the American Psychiatric Association (APA) and non-APA affiliated organizations interested in DSM development (APA, 2000). Despite the expectation of an "extensive empirical foundation" (APA 2004, p. xxiii) for DSM-IV diagnostic categories, some authors have been critical of the DSM process (Caplan, 1991) and, according to Caplan (2004), even the DSM's own subcommittee on LLPDD concluded that "(1) very little research supported the existence of a premenstrual illness [that could be separated from the physical signs associated with PMS]; and (2) the most relevant research was preliminary and methodologically flawed" (Caplan, 2004, p. 58). Quoting from their personal communication with a member of the PMDD Work Group, Frank and Severino (1995) wrote: "... we have taken a vague diagnosis, validated it by instruments designed to find it and then announced that this population exists" (p. 25).

EVALUATING THE EMPIRICAL FOUNDATION OF PMD

Some authors have challenged inclusion of LLPDD in the DSM, even as an unspecified disorder requiring further study, because "... few epidemiological studies have documented true incidence and prevalence of LLPDD in the general population" (Blumenthal & Nadelson, 1988, p. 471), Gehlert, Chang & Hartlage (1999) voiced similar concerns about the empirical basis for PMDD: "Factor analysis of the group of women who met the diagnosis of PMDD was not possible. Few women meet the diagnosis of PMDD when the criteria set forth in the DSM-IV are adhered to strictly" (p. 78).

Despite extensive research and many clinical reports, "proof" of menstrual cycle-related condition that could be defined as an illness has not been provided (Caplan, 2004). As noted by Halbriech (1993) in a review of earlier literature,
 It is disheartening that 40 years after the panel
 And 60 years after Frank's, 1931, pioneering
 reports on PMT, literally dozens of papers
 include statements like "the etiology of
 Premenstrual syndrome, PMS, is still
 obscure" (p. 25).


While there are many conditions, accepted as such, for which the aetiology is not known, it may also be, as Walker (1992) suggests, that problems of documentation arise
 ... because researchers and clinicians are
 attempting to define and study a phenomenon
 which does not exist. The lack of
 comparability between studies, which is
 attributed to methodological factors, could also
 be attributed to an attempt to impose a
 hypothetical syndrome on behavioral and
 emotional changes which are not related to
 reproductive functioning (p.75).


As noted previously, this is not to say that women do not experience difficult symptoms associated with their menstrual cycles, but rather that the diagnosis has been and remains in question (Caplan et al., 1992; Caplan, 2004).

PMDD DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA

The DSM criteria for diagnosing PMDD are intended to serve as a template to conduct further studies by creating a unified research base from which to understand the disorder. Contrary to this, Breaux, Hartlage and Gelhert (2000) point out that very few studies have applied all of the diagnostic criteria. Two of the diagnostic criteria that deserve particular attention are comorbidity and the measurement of impairment of functioning.

Comorbidity. Diagnosing PMDD is complicated when women who present with PMDD also have a history of mood, personality and anxiety disorders, substance abuse and exacerbated symptoms of ongoing mood disorders premenstrually (Breaux et al., 2000; Steiner & Born, 2000). Although the PMDD criteria explicitly state that PMDD can be diagnosed only when other disorders are not present, it is unclear how often PMDD occurs alone. For example, Steiner and Born (2000) estimated PMDD to range from 3-8% of women, but none of the literature reviewed clearly specified rates of Pure-Pure PMDD (no past or present psychiatric disorders) or Pure PMDD (no current psychiatric disorder, but history of psychiatric disorders). However, Wittchen, Becker, Lieb & Krause (2002) found high rates of comorbidity between PMDD and other mental disorders. The authors point out that their sample of young adults (15- to 24-year-olds followed for 4 years) missed a second group over age 35 thought to have high incidence of PMDD based on previous literature.

Hartlage and Gehlert (2001) reported, "Differentiating PMDD from premenstrual exacerbation of other disorders has proven to be difficult methodologically" (p. 250). This statement suggests that it may be difficult to conduct research on PMDD without confounding the results with the effects of other mood disorders. Further complicating the issue, Halbriech and Endicott (1987) report that prospective ratings of premenstrual symptoms correlated better with past mood disturbance than with PMS. McMillan and Pihl (1987) also found that women who sought treatment for PMS often had premenstrual exacerbation of depression or dysthymia and not PMS. Moreover, many of the symptoms of PMDD overlap with those of PMS (Chrisler & Caplan, 2002), despite the fact that the DSM states that PMDD should be distinguished from PMS (APA, 2000).

Impairment of Functioning. The DSM states that in order for PMDD to be diagnosed, there must be a marked interference in a woman' s regular behaviour including the areas of work, school, usual social activities and relationships. This is known as Criterion B for diagnosis. However, few studies have looked at the impact of PMDD symptoms on functioning (Freeman & Halbreich, 1998) and "Few investigators have reported evidence of impaired functioning in women diagnosed with PMDD. Instead, impairment is assumed when symptoms are rated as severe" (Hartlage & Gehlert, 2001, p. 249). This assumption of impairment makes the diagnostic criteria arbitrary. It is based upon the researchers' standard as opposed to the woman's. Often researchers do not state how they measured or defined impairment of functioning in areas of work, school, usual social activities and relationships for the women in the study. For example, Gehlert, Chang and Hartlage (1999) state that they included only symptoms that occurred on days when functioning was impaired, but they do not state how they measured impaired functioning. In an earlier report dealing with PMS, MacFarlane and Williams (1990) noted similarly that determining whether or not there have been "marked" changes in functioning is often left up to the researcher or clinician and does not take into account the variability between women, thereby making the criteria arbitrary. More recently, Smith, Schmidt and Rubinow (2003) observed that the interference criterion (Criterion B) does not contribute to the ability to diagnose PMDD.

When impairment in functioning is measured, a change in symptoms from the luteal score to follicular score is sometimes used as a measure of functioning. According to Steiner and Born (2000), a within-cycle change in symptoms is Calculated by subtracting follicular scores from luteal scores, dividing by the luteal score and then multiplying by 100 to get a percentage change rating. A change of 50% from follicular to luteal phase scores is suggested for diagnosis. This system of diagnosis is unsatisfactory. The lack of agreed-upon measures for PMDD is controversial (Rodin, 1992). Currently, there are no questionnaires that measure levels of psychosocial functioning that are specific to PMDD (Freeman & Halbreich, 1998). The diagnosing of PMDD is therefore complicated by the lack of objective diagnostic tests and consensus among investigators/ clinicians concerning the reliability of instruments for confirming PMDD prospectively (Steiner & Born, 2000). Although the Moos Menstrual Distress Questionnaire (MDQ) is often used, there remain problems with the test due to its use of only closed ended questions (McFarlane & Williams, 1990) as well as its previously reported lack of reliability or external consistency data (Parlee, 1973).

The 50% indiscriminate cut-off level also calls into question the usefulness of the PMDD category for women presenting with a range of symptoms. Although this may be a useful tool for identifying large groups of women quickly for research purposes, it represents an arbitrary cut-off point that may not be sensitive enough to distinguish the needs of an individual woman (MacFarlane & Williams, 1990). Even if a 50% cut-off level is not applied in a clinical setting, the process of diagnosing women with PMDD based on marked change in functioning is highly subjective. As Caplan (2004) points out, the subjective nature of the diagnosing of PMDD is in conflict with the APA's descriptions of the DSM's "... impressive record of the manual in ensuring readability and uniformity among therapists in their decisions about who meets the criteria for PMDD" (p. 59).

THE SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION OF PMDD

The preceding review of research on menstrual cycle-related changes highlight methodological questions about the PMDD diagnosis and about its continuing placement in the DSM. The following section examines the social construction of PMDD within the context of medical discourse on PMDD, the associated interests of pharmaceutical companies in creating drugs to treat PMDD, and the influence of these factors on women's experiences of their bodies. We suggest that PMDD is a socially constructed disorder rather than a psychiatric disorder.

THE CONSTRUCTION OF A MEDICAL DISCOURSE ON PMDD

The history of PMDD developed within a medical context of women's reproductive health (Nicolson, 1995; Rodin, 1992). As discussed earlier, this has led to the medicalization of women's experiences of menstrual cycle-related changes. Juxtaposed to this, a strong feminist critique exists on the medical understanding of menstrual cycle-related changes. As Rodin (1992) states in a report on the social construction of PMS:
 The fact that the medical establishment treats
 PMS as a legitimate disease category (by
 applying for research funds, proceeding with
 research, treating patients, and maintaining
 PMS clinics) despite the lack of an agreed
 upon definition and contradictory research
 findings, suggests that shared cultural
 knowledge, as opposed to scientific facts,
 informs researcher understanding of what
 constitutes PMS (p.52).


Oudshoorn (1994) suggests that much of what is known of the female body are social constructs defined through scientific research. She argues that scientific facts are created in a social context and become embedded in popular beliefs. Nevertheless, health researchers continue to minimize the role of social influences on their work (Caplan et al., 1992) and proponents of the DSM overlook the fact that psychiatric disorders are cultural constructs (Chrisler & Caplan, 2002).

Often it is assumed, from the use of predominant research paradigms, that PMDD is a valid and well-established category of mental disorder. Our culture bases much of its belief system on the quest for knowledge through scientific, positivist, explorations making it hard to question scientific "facts." This use of positivist research methods has dominated premenstrual research and has lead to the existence of PMDD as a "taken-for-granted-fact" (Ussher, 1996). But to the extent that scientific knowledge is constructed, there exists a reciprocal relationship between scientific and social beliefs (Oudshoom, 1994). Rodin (1992) upholds this view when she states, "... PMS is a recreation of tacit cultural knowledge about the effect of the reproduction system on women's behaviors as value free scientific fact" (p. 50).

The relation between scientific and social beliefs is strengthened when discoveries about norms and disorders become news stories and are incorporated into common sense beliefs about the world and a person's own relationship to psychological norms (Nicolson, 1995). Cosgrove and Riddle (in press) speak to this relationship when they state, "In print ads in all of the major women's magazines, in television spots, and on websites, women are being inundated with 'expert knowledge' that encourages them to understand experiences of negative affect as PMDD and to take advantage of the 'new' treatment that has been developed ..." (p. 4). Notions of the premenstrual body and unpleasant emotional changes have become logical and natural in our society (Rodin, 1992). Pharmaceutical companies have become major stakeholders in this process. They have become involved by contributing research money to the studies on the treatment of PMDD (Cosgrove & Riddle, 2003) and they have used the mass media to persuade women that their experiences of menstrual cycle-related changes require treatment/medication (Caplan, 2004).

THE ROLE OF PHARMACEUTICALS

Tavris (1992) points out that it was not until the media started reporting that PMS was costing billions of dollars per year in lost productivity that it became known as a "disorder" or "disease". With this label came a thriving industry focused on a cure. The support of psychiatric research by pharmaceutical companies enables clinical research in the area of PMDD and the development of PMDD validates the use of pharmaceutical treatments in the management ofpremenstrual symptoms. By identifying PMDD as a psychiatric disorder, a pathological condition has been constructed which demands clinical intervention and treatment. According to Caplan (2004), a strong relationship has thus been developed between the legitimacy of PMDD as a mental disorder, the prescription and marketing of fluoxetine for treatment, and social beliefs about the menstrual cycle. Caplan (2002) notes that the only psychiatric therapy for PMDD is fluoxetine and women have been asking for the drug they have seen advertised in the media to treat PMDD.

When PMDD was moved into the "Depressive Disorders not Otherwise Specified" section of the DSM-IV the editors of the DSM reinforced the view that PMDD is depression (Frank & Severino, 1995). The fact that fluoxetine is a treatment for depression, and now the only approved treatment for PMDD, further reifies this connection. Gold (1994) suggests that this connection could provide further clarification of the PMDD diagnostic entity: "It is hoped the results of studies demonstrating the effectiveness of antidepressant medications will assist researchers in their search for the etiology underlying premenstrual complaints and elucidate why some women note premenstrual changes and others become ill" (p. 1025). If the PMDD-depression connection is made, is it not possible that fluoxetine is treating depressed women rather than women with PMDD? Indeed Caplan (2004) argues
 Certainly, if fluoxetine is given to any group
 of depressed or upset people, some will feel
 better. But that reveals nothing about the
 causes of their upset and, in this case
 [PMDD], it doesn't provide that the upset is
 related to menstrual cycle-related changes
 (p. 61).


The linking of PMDD with depression, while legitimizing of the use of fluoxetine to relieve symptoms, provides little information on the nature of the disorder and raises the problem of comorbidity discussed earlier in the paper.

DEFINING WOMEN'S EXPERIENCES OF PMDD

Research in the area of menstrual cycle-related changes has had a major influence on the social construction of premenstrual diagnostic categories (Johnson, 1987). It can be argued that research has primed women and society, for the last 70 years, to believe in and expect menstrual complications. Indeed, research has found that women's perceptions of what happens in the premenstruum becomes more pathological when they are introduced to the diagnostic definition of PMDD (Hash & Chrisler, 1997). This link is exemplified in research that highlights and recreates social expectations of premenstrual experiences. For example, pharmaceutical companies sponsor research conferences and educational seminars on premenstrual symptoms, media coverage follows, and women are reinforced in their perceptions of their condition and in use of medications as a treatment (Tavris, 1992).

The category of PMDD can be thought of as an extension of long-held social beliefs that place an emphasis on abnormality and discontent between women and their bodies (Ussher, 1996). If a diagnostic category only applies to one gender and is based in gender stereotypes, there is a possibility that it will lead to discrimination and bias (Chrisler & Nash, 1997). Indeed, Blake (1995) states, "The media present PMS as a medical label explaining almost any fluctuating disturbance of a woman's well being in the reproductive years. This labelling is influenced by cultural assumptions about the role and behavior of women ..." (p. 168). When the language surrounding menstrual cycle-related changes is pathologized, it becomes difficult for women to explain their experiences outside of a medical discourse. The biomedical model validates these feelings, taking women seriously because of their "medical condition" and this can be empowering (Tavris, 1992).

McFarlane, Martin and Williams (1988) suggest that negative bias relating to women's menstrual-cycle related changes is subtle and complex. They explain
 It seems unlikely that women are complete
 dupes of socially-produced expectations.
 Perhaps most women do not usually
 Experience mood problems, but occasionally,
 if a number of other factors converge (and
 those factors may vary for individual
 women), they feel or behave in ways that
 are out of character (p. 218).


They further suggest that when negative moods coincide with the premenstruum, even if this happens occasionally, this would be sufficient to maintain the belief in PMS because people selectively attend to information congruent with their stereotypes. Indeed, Chrisler and Johnston-Robledo (2002) suggest that research should begin to look at the stresses women experience in their lives as possible contributors to experiences of menstrual cycle-related changes.

THE LINK BETWEEN PMDD ANN THE MEDICALIZATION OF FEMALE SEXUALITY

It can be argued that the inclusion of PMDD in the DSM contributes to the further medical classification of women's sexuality. In opposition to this, the medicalization of women's experiences of their sexuality is beginning to be challenged and the search for a new understanding of women's sexuality has begun to develop. This alternate view point provides an important forum for debate over the construction of women's experiences of their menstrual bodies. A full issue of Women and Therapy: A Feminist Quarterly was devoted to "A New View of Women's Sexual Problems" (2001), which challenges the current medical view of women's sexuality. The authors of the title article state, "We believe that a fundamental barrier to understanding women's sexuality is the medical classification scheme in current use, developed by the American Psychiatric Association [APA] for its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Disorders (DSM) ..." (The Working Group For A New View of Women's Sexual Problems, 2001, p. 2).

Experiences of menstrual cycle-related changes have been linked to female sexuality and its expression through sexual scripts. Gagnon (1990) states, "What has been confused in this debate [on gender and sexual behavior] is the difference between reproductive conduct, gender conduct and sexual conduct" (pp. 5). How young women are taught about menstruation influences not only their beliefs about menstruation but also how they learn about other aspects of their sexuality (Beausang & Razor, 2000). Unfortunately, many young girls are influenced by cultural beliefs that menstruation is associated with negative physical effect, emotional instability and negative thoughts (Koff & Rierdan, 1995). These influences can be powerful given the fact that discussions on some sexual areas, including menstruation, are still taboo in North American culture (Beausang & Razor, 2000) leaving young women unable to question these beliefs. A society that helps young girls to get a basic understanding of their physical functioning and offers them information they need to make healthy decisions about their sexuality, will foster their ability to own their sexual/reproductive experiences.

If young women are not able to own their own sexual/ reproductive experiences, who does? Unfortunately, this lack of personal ownership and accompanying sense of disconnection is exacerbated by the medical discourse surrounding women's bodily experiences leading to the medical ownership of women's experiences and ultimately the medicalization of these experiences (Kleinplatz, 2001). One example of medicalization leading to discontent is seen in the possible sexual side effects experienced by women who take SSRIs for treatment of PMDD. Steiner (2000) lists the most troublesome side effects of SSRI treatment, including delayed or absent orgasm.

The link between menstrual cycle-related changes and sexuality is not entirely negative. Stewart (1989) states that sexual arousal, pleasure and orgasm are high during the premenstrual period, and that sexual responsiveness actually increases during ovulation and during the premenstruum. Halbriech (1993) mentions that there can be an increase in energy prior to menstruation. Ussher (1996) states that cultural restrictions on the expression of female sexuality have been cited as a possible cause of adverse menstrual cycle-related changes. The positive aspects of menstrual cycle-related changes warrant more research attention as one aspect of a movement toward comprehending the female body as cyclical, fluctuating and normal and away from research that further medicalizes women's experiences of their bodies.

References

Ackermann R., & Williams, J.W. (2002). Rational treatment choices for non-major depressions in primacy care, and evidence-based review. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 17, 293-301.

Alberts, P., & Alberts, M.. (1990). Unvalidated treatment of premenstrual syndrome. International Journal of Mental Health, 19, 68-80.

American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR. Washington, DC: The American Psychiatric Association.

Beausang, C., & Razor, A. (2000). Young western women's experiences of menarche and menstruation. Health Care for Women International, 21, 517-528.

Blake, F. (1995). Cognitive therapy for premenstrual syndrome. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 2, 167-185.

Blumenthal, S., & Nadelson, C. (1988). Late luteal phase dysphoric disorder (premenstrual syndromes): Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 49, 469-474.

Breaux, C., Hartlage, S., & Gehlert, S. (2000). Relationships of premenstrual dysphoric disorder to major depression and anxiety disorders: A re-examination. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetric Gynecology, 21, 17-24.

Caplan, P. (1991). How do they decide who is normal? The bizarre, but true, tale of the DSM process. Canadian Psychology, 32, 162-170.

Caplan, P. (2004). The debate about PMDD and Sarafem: Suggestions for therapists. Women & Therapy, A Feminist Quarterly, 27, 55-67.

Caplan, P., McCurdy-Myers, J., & Gans, M. (1992). Should "Premenstrual Syndrome" be called a psychiatric abnormality? Feminism & Psychology, 2, 27-44.

Chrisler, J., & Caplan, P. (2002). The strange case of Dr. Jekyll and Ms. Hyde: How PMS became a cultural phenomenon and a psychiatric disorder. Annual Review of Sex Research, 13, 272-306.

Chrisler, J., & Johnston-Robledo, I.K. (2002). Raging Hormones? Feminist perspectives on premenstrual syndrome and postpartum depression. In M. Ballou & L.S. Brown (Eds.), Rethinking Mental Health and Disorder (pp. 174-197). New York, NY: Guilford.

Conrad, P., & Schneider, J. (1980). Deviance and Medicalization: From Badness to Sickness. St. Louis, MO: C.V. Mosby.

Cosgrove, L., & Riddle, B. (2003). Constructions of femininity and experiences of menstrual distress. Women and Health, 38, 37-58.

Dalton, K., & Greene, R. (1953). The premenstrual syndrome. British Medical Journal, 1, 1007-1014.

Davis, L., & Yonkers, K. (1997). Diagnosis and treatment Of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 1, 149-156.

Endicott, J. (2000). History, evolution and diagnosis of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61(12), 5-8.

Figert, A. (1996). Women and the Ownership of PMS. New York, NY: Walter de Gruyter, Inc.

Fink, P. (1987). (Introduction) In B.E. Ginsburg, & B.F. Carter (Eds.), Premenstrual Syndrome Ethical and Legal Implications in a Biomedical Perspective. New York: Plenum Press.

Frank, E., & Severino, S. (1995). Premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Facts/meanings. Journal of Practical Psychiatric Behavioural Health, 1, 20-28.

Frank, R. (1931). The hormonal causes of premenstrual tension. Archives Neurological Psychiatry, 26, 1053-1057.

Freeman, E.W., & Halbreich, U. (1998). Premenstrual syndromes. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 34, 291-295.

Freeman, E.W., & Sondheimer, S.J. (2003). Premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Recognition and treatment. Primary Care Companion Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 5, 30-39.

Gagnon, S. (1990). The explicit and implicit use of the scripting perspective in sex research. Annual Review of Sex Research, 1, 1-43.

Gehlert, S., Chang, C., & Hartlage, S. (1999). Symptom patterns of premenstrual dysphoric disorder as defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV. Journal of Women's Health, 8, 75-85.

Gold, J. (1994). Historical perspectives of premenstrual syndrome. In J.H. Gold & S.K. Severino (Eds.), Premenstrual Dysphorias: Myths and Realities (pp. 171-183). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press.

Halbreich, U. (1993). Menstrually related changes and disorders: Conceptualization and diagnostic considerations. Neuropsychopharmacology, 9, 25-29.

Halbreich, U., & Endicott, J. (1987). Dysphoric premenstrual changes: Are they related to affective disorders? In B. Ginsburg, & K. Carter (Eds)., Premenstrual Syndrome (pp. 351-367). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Hartlage, S., & Gehlert, S. (2001). Differentiating premenstrual dysphoric disorder from premenstrual exacerbations of other disorders: A methods dilemma. Clinical Psychology." Science And Practice, 8, 242-253.

Johnson, T. (1987). Premenstrual syndrome as a western culture-specific disorder. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 11, 337-356.

Kleinplatz, P.J. (2001). On the outside looking in: In search of women's sexual experience. Journal of Women and Therapy, 24, 123-132.

Koff, E., & Rierdan, J. (1995). Preparing girls for menstruation: Recommendations from adolescent girls. Adolescence, 30, 795-811.

McFarlane, J., Martin, C., & Williams, T. (1988). Mood fluctuations: Women versus men and menstrual versus other cycles. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 201-223.

McFarlane, J. & Williams, T. (1990). The enigma of premenstrual syndrome. Canadian Psychology, 31, 95-108.

McMiUan, M., & Pihl, R. (1987). Premenstrual depression: A distinct entity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 149-154.

Nash, H., & Chrisler, J. (1997). Is a little (psychiatric) knowledge a dangerous thing? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 315-322.

Nicolson, P. (1995). The menstrual cycle, science and femininity: Assumptions underlying menstrual cycle research. Social Science and Medicine, 41, 779-784.

Olasov, B. & Jackson, J. (1987). Effects of expectations on women's reports of moods during the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 65-78.

Oudshoorn, N. (1994). Beyond the Natural Body: An archaeology of sex hormones. New York, NY: Routledge.

Parlee, M. (1973). The premenstrual syndrome. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 454-465.

Parlee, M. (1987). Media treatment of premenstrual syndrome. In B.E. Ginsburg & B.F. Carter (Eds.), Premenstrual Syndrome: Ethical and Legal Implication in a Biomedical Perspective, (pp. 189-205). New York, NY: Plenum.

Pearlstein, T., Frank, E., Rivera-Tovar, A., Thoft, J., Jacobs, E., & Mieczkowski, T. (1990). Prevalence of axis I and axis II disorders in women with late luteal phase dysphoric disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 20, 129-134.

Richardson, J. (1995). The premenstrual syndrome: A brief history. Social Science and Medicine, 41, 761-767.

Rodin, M. (1992). The social construction of premenstrual syndrome. Social Science and Medicine, 35, 49-56.

Rubinow, D., & Roy-Byrne, P. (1984). Premenstrual syndrome: Overview from a methodological perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 163-172.

Schnurr, P., Hurt, S., & Stout, A. (1994). Consequences of methodological decisions on the diagnosis of late luteal phase dysphoric disorder. In J. Gold, & S. Severmo (Eds.), Premenstrual Dysphorias: Myths and Realities (pp. 19-46). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Severino, S. (1996). Premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Controversies surrounding the diagnosis. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 293-295.

Smith, M., Schmidt, D., & Rubinow, D. (2003). Operationalizing DSM-IV criteria for PMDD: Selecting symptomatic and asymptomatic cycles for research. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 75-83.

Steiner, M. (2000). Premenstrual syndrome and premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Guidelines for management. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 25, 459-468.

Steiner, M., & Born, L. (2000). Advances in the diagnosis and treatment of premenstrual dysphoria. Central Nervous System Drugs, 13, 287-304.

Stewart, D. (1989). Positive changes in the premenstrual syndrome. Acta Psychiatric Scandinavia, 79, 400-405.

Tavris, C. (1992). The Mismeasure of Woman. New York, NY: Touchstone.

Tiefer, L. (2002). Beyond the medical model of women's sexual problems: A campaign to resist the promotion of "female sexual dysfunction." Sexual and Relationship Theory, 17, 127-135.

The Working Group for a New View of Women's Sexual Problems. (2001). A new view of women's sexual problems. Women and Therapy: A Feminist Quarterly, 24, 1-8.

Ussher, J. (1996). Premenstrual syndrome: Reconciling disciplinary divides through the adoption of material-discursive epistemological standpoint. Annual Review of Sex Research, 7, 218-251.

Ussher, J. (2002). Process of appraisal and coping in the development and maintenance of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 309-322.

Walker, A. (1992). Premenstrual symptoms and ovarian hormones: A review. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 10, 67-82.

Wittchen, H., Becker, E., Lieb, R., & Krause, P. (2002). Prevalence, incidence and stability of premenstrual dysphoric disorder in the community. Psychological Medicine, 32, 119-132.

Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Alia Offman, Ph. D., 97 Pigeon Terrace, Ottawa, ON K1V 9H6.

Alia Offman

Carleton University

Ottawa, Ontario

Peggy J. Kleinplatz

University of Ottawa

Ottawa, Ontario
COPYRIGHT 2004 SIECCAN, The Sex Information and Education Council of Canada
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2004 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

 Reader Opinion

Title:

Comment:



 

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Offman, Alia; Kleinplatz, Peggy J.
Publication:The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality
Geographic Code:1CANA
Date:Mar 22, 2004
Words:6439
Previous Article:Sexual health education: attitudes, knowledge, and comfort of teachers in New Brunswick Schools.
Next Article:Biobehavioural and cognitive determinants of adolescent girls' involvement in sexual risk behaviours: a test of three theoretical models.
Topics:


Related Articles
Intermittent dosing okayed for PMDD regimens. (Limited to Luteal Phase).
Paxil CR.
Luteal-phase treatment may be best for PMDD.
Luteal-phase treatment may be best for PMDD.
Severe premenstrual syndrome can affect stress responses.
Investigational OC may ease PMDD symptoms.
Investigational OC may decrease PMDD symptoms: regimen of 24 days of low-dose drospirenone tied to fewer symptoms during the pill-free interval.

Terms of use | Copyright © 2014 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters