Printer Friendly
The Free Library
22,725,466 articles and books

Cognition, metacognition, and achievement of college students with learning disabilities.



Abstract. This study examined the way successful college students with LD compensated for their deficits in phonological pho·nol·o·gy  
n. pl. pho·nol·o·gies
1. The study of speech sounds in language or a language with reference to their distribution and patterning and to tacit rules governing pronunciation.

2.
 processing. Successful was defined as average or above-average grades in college coursework. The study compared the cognitive and metacognitive performance of students with and without LD (N=40). Although achievement levels for both groups were comparable, students with LD scored significantly lower than students without LD in word reading, processing speed See MHz. , semantic processing, and short-term memory short-term memory
n.
Abbr. STM The phase of the memory process in which stimuli that have been recognized and registered are stored briefly.
. Differences were also found between groups in self-regulation and number of hours of studying. Results showed that students with LD compensated for their processing deficits by relying on verbal abilities, learning strategies, and help seeking.

**********

Despite their persisting academic difficulties, adults with learning disabilities (LD) are enrolling in postsecondary education in increasing numbers (Newman & Cameto 1993; Henderson, 1999; Wagner & Blackorby, 1996). A major impetus behind this growth has been legislation and educational reform in K-12 settings. These changes in public policy, in turn, have increased funding, services, and efforts to identify and serve students with LD in school, thus producing more high-school graduates who are eligible for college entrance. Parallel to these developments, postsecondary institutions now grapple with the issues of serving college students with LD, guided by section 504 of the Rehabilitation rehabilitation: see physical therapy.  Act (1977) (as quoted in Adelman & Vogel, 1993) and the Americans with Disabilities Act Americans with Disabilities Act, U.S. civil-rights law, enacted 1990, that forbids discrimination of various sorts against persons with physical or mental handicaps.  (ADA Ada, city, United States
Ada (ā`ə), city (1990 pop. 15,820), seat of Pontotoc co., S central Okla.; inc. 1904. It is a large cattle market and the center of a rich oil and ranch area.
) of 1990. As a result of the increased efficacy of K-12 education for students with LD, this group is now the largest group of college students with disabilities (Henderson, 1999).

Research suggests that college students with LD have significant difficulties in multiple academic domains (e.g., Vogel & Adelman, 1992; Wilczenski, 1993). Forexample, reports on students with LD indicate problems in reading, writing, math, and foreign language. They are at risk for failure in their courses and at increased risk for dropping out of college beyond the freshman year compared with their nonlearning disabled (NLD NLD
abbr.
nonverbal learning disorder
) peers (Vogel & Adelman, 1992; Wilczenski, 1993). At the same time, however, studies on colleges with adequate academic support show that students with LD can attain normative levels of achievement (McGuire, Hall, & Litt, 1991; Vogel & Adelman, 1992). The question is how do students with these difficulties compensate for their deficits?

One of the ways college students with LD may compensate for their cognitive difficulties is by relying on metacognition Metacognition refers to thinking about cognition (memory, perception, calculation, association, etc.) itself or to think/reason about one's own thinking. Types of knowledge ; that is, consciously controlling actions that are too complex to be controlled automatically. Metacognition is defined by Zimmerman (1986) as the ability to adjust behavioral, environmental functioning in response to changing academic demands. Pintrich (1994) defines academic metacognition as a construct comprised of three major elements: (a) active control over learning-related behaviors such as when, how much, and with whom a student is learning; (b) self-regulation of motivation and affect, in which students learn how to control their emotions and even use them in goal setting; (c) control over various cognitive strategies for learning, such as rehearsal and memory strategies.

Use of metacognitive strategies may be linked to efficient ways to improve performance in academic and work environments. For example, young adults in college regulate their learning behavior with a variety of specific strategies (Pintrich, 1994). They manage their own time, decide with whom to study, and monitor their comprehension with a variety of internal (self-regulation, strategies) and external (peers, family, faculty) supports.

In addition, several studies have shown that metacognition is not a set of idiosyncratic id·i·o·syn·cra·sy  
n. pl. id·i·o·syn·cra·sies
1. A structural or behavioral characteristic peculiar to an individual or group.

2. A physiological or temperamental peculiarity.

3.
 behaviors but a finite set In mathematics, a set is called finite if there is a bijection between the set and some set of the form where n is a natural number. (The value n = 0 is allowed; that is, the empty set is finite.) An infinite set is a set which is not finite.  of common skills that are highly correlated to academic success (Garcia & Pintrich, 1994, Pintrich, 1994). Reis, McGuire, and New (2000) described qualitative patterns focusing mainly on motivational aspects in support of the hypothesis that metacognition plays a major role in the success of students with LD. Recent research on college students with LD suggests that metacognition (e.g., self-regulated behavior) is a strong predictor of academic success (Ruban, 2000; Smitely, 2001).

In explaining the compensatory role of metacognition in the academic achievement of students with LD, an analogy may be drawn to research on the relationship between metacognition and cognitive deficits Cognitive deficit is an inclusive term to describe any characteristic that acts as a barrier to cognitive performance. The term may describe deficits in global intellectual performance, such as mental retardation, or it may describe specific deficits in cognitive abilities  in the literature on aging. While older adults face diminishing cognitive abilities and reduction in efficiency (Salthouse, 1992), some fare much better than others for a longer time (Dixon & Baeckman, 1995). Baltes and Baltes (1990) suggested three possible paths to compensating for lost cognitive abilities. One such path is metacognitive control, defined as strategies, effort, and self-regulation of activities. This path must be under the direct control of the individual. That is, the person faced with a task related to a deficit must decide to use a strategy to compensate while weighing costs and benefits. This includes strategies related to the individual's actions (e.g., making lists to compensate for memory deficit), as well as strategies that include technological supports and help seeking. Another path, cognitive compensation, implies that individuals who face a deficit in one cognitive domain cognitive domain,
n area of study that deals with the processes and measurable results of study, as well as the practical ability to apply intelligence.
 may use relative strengths from another cognitive domain such as general processing, working memory, or semantic ability that can assist performance. Finally, Dixon and Baeckman (1995) offer a third option, lowering of standards. Here, the individual chooses an alternative approach that reduces the demands on the deficient process; for example, writing shorter sentences to reduce working memory load (Kemper, 1986).

In this study we considered whether successful students with LD rely on metacognitive strategies to compensate for processing difficulties. We also considered the deficient cognitive processes Cognitive processes
Thought processes (i.e., reasoning, perception, judgment, memory).

Mentioned in: Psychosocial Disorders
 that metacognition may compensate for. The literature suggests that students with LD in most cases have deficits in phonological processing. For example, comparing the phonological processing of adult LD with age-and-reading-level matched groups, Bruck (1992) found that adults with childhood diagnoses of LD functioned far below the norm for both their age and reading level. Similarly, Scarborough (1984) reported that adults with a childhood history of dyslexia dyslexia (dĭslĕk`sēə), in psychology, a developmental disability in reading or spelling, generally becoming evident in early schooling. To a dyslexic, letters and words may appear reversed, e.g.  performed poorly on non-word spelling tasks, confirming the persistence of phonological encoding See encode.  difficulties. A study by Pennington and his colleagues (1986) also revealed a deficit in phonological processing in adults with LD, while showing normative performance in their ability to spell morphologically complex patterns. In a subsequent study, Lefly and Pennington (1991) found that the same pattern of low phonology phonology, study of the sound systems of languages. It is distinguished from phonetics, which is the study of the production, perception, and physical properties of speech sounds; phonology attempts to account for how they are combined, organized, and convey meaning  in adults with LD was compensated for by spelling. Although not age-appropriate, spelling of morphologically complex words was better than phonological spellings.

Considerable evidence shows that the word reading ability of adults with LD is consistently lower than that of their nondisabled peers (Apthorp, 1995; Bruck, 1990; Ritter rit·ter  
n. pl. ritter
A knight.



[German, from Middle High German riter, from Middle Dutch ridder, from r
, 2000). While the reading comprehension Reading comprehension can be defined as the level of understanding of a passage or text. For normal reading rates (around 200-220 words per minute) an acceptable level of comprehension is above 75%.  of college students with LD was considerably lower than that of their NLD peers, it was still considerably higher than that of decoding de·code  
tr.v. de·cod·ed, de·cod·ing, de·codes
1. To convert from code into plain text.

2. To convert from a scrambled electronic signal into an interpretable one.

3.
 level-matched (hence younger) participants (Bruck, 1985, 1990; Ransby & Swanson, 2003). Furthermore, comprehension of both reading and listening has been shown to be orthogonal At right angles. The term is used to describe electronic signals that appear at 90 degree angles to each other. It is also widely used to describe conditions that are contradictory, or opposite, rather than in parallel or in sync with each other.  to reading fluency and word reading ability in LD students (Ransby & Swanson, 2003). This finding suggests that college students with LD may rely on different cognitive processes (e.g., processes related to listening comprehension rather than phonological decoding) in their path to reading comprehension compared to their NED peers.

In summary, the purpose of this study was to explore whether college students identified with LD use metacognitive strategies to compensate for their phonological processing deficits. Two student groups (LD vs. NLD) were compared to determine those processes that are similar and those that are different between the two groups. We hypothesized that college students with disabilities compensate in cognitive processing by relying on metacognitive strategies. We also explored whether such students rely on strengths in other cognitive processes in addition to metacognition that allow them to compensate for difficulties in phonological processing.

METHOD

Participants

Participants were 40 students (20 LD, 20 without LD) from four universities in Southern California Southern California, also colloquially known as SoCal, is the southern portion of the U.S. state of California. Centered on the cities of Los Angeles and San Diego, Southern California is home to nearly 24 million people and is the nation's second most populated region, . Students with LD volunteered for the study after a letter soliciting their participation was sent by the disability centers in the respective universities. Students without LD (referred to as NLD) were matched as closely as possible to the population of students with LD on demographic variables of ethnicity, college major, gender, and academic standing (i.e., grades). Participants' chronological age chron·o·log·i·cal age
n. Abbr. CA
The number of years a person has lived, used especially in psychometrics as a standard against which certain variables, such as behavior and intelligence, are measured.
 ranged from 18 to 64 years. The NLD group had a mean age of 21.75 (SD=1.12), while the group with LD had a mean age of 31.4 (SD=13.56). The LD group consisted of 15 women and 5 men. The control group (NLD) consisted of 16 women and 4 men.

Overall ethnic composition was 40% White non-Hispanic, 20% Hispanic, 12.5% African American African American Multiculture A person having origins in any of the black racial groups of Africa. See Race. , 22.5% Asian American A·sian A·mer·i·can also A·sian-A·mer·i·can  
n.
A U.S. citizen or resident of Asian descent. See Usage Note at Amerasian.



A
, and 5% other or mixed ethnicities. The distribution of ethnicity in the two groups was not significantly different, [chi square chi square (kī),
n a nonparametric statistic used with discrete data in the form of frequency count (nominal data) or percentages or proportions that can be reduced to frequencies.
] (4)=3.95, p=.41. Class standing in the LD and NLD groups was similar, as illustrated in Table 1. Further, there was no difference between the distributions of major fields of study between the groups.

Participants' socioeconomic status socioeconomic status,
n the position of an individual on a socio-economic scale that measures such factors as education, income, type of occupation, place of residence, and in some populations, ethnicity and religion.
 was measured by parental education. The NLD group had a higher level of parental education, M=5.53 (SD=1.29) (5=some postsecondary education and 6=undergraduate degree “First degree” redirects here. For the BBC television series, see First Degree.

An undergraduate degree (sometimes called a first degree or simply a degree
), compared to students with LD (M=4.22 (SD=2.15). The difference was statistically significant t(38)=2.32, p=.027, d=.73. Most of the difference between the LD and NLD groups may be attributed to four participants (20%) in the LD group who came from immigrant families and whose parents had either an elementary education elementary education
 or primary education

Traditionally, the first stage of formal education, beginning at age 5–7 and ending at age 11–13.
 or no formal education at all. The observed difference corresponds to the data on young adults with disabilities presented by Wagner and her colleagues (1993).

Selection

Students with LD were recruited with cooperation from the university disability centers at participating institutions. The participants with LD had been either diagnosed or had their diagnosis confirmed in adulthood by trained professionals. Further, the LD diagnosis was confirmed by experimental measures in this study. To be considered LD a participant had to satisfy two conditions on standardized measures: (a) low phonological processing (< 25th percentile percentile,
n the number in a frequency distribution below which a certain percentage of fees will fall. E.g., the ninetieth percentile is the number that divides the distribution of fees into the lower 90% and the upper 10%, or that fee level
) and (b) average range of intelligence (IQ > 84). All the students who were previously identified as having LD satisfied both conditions. Descriptive statistics descriptive statistics

see statistics.
 for both groups are shown in Table 2. Success in college was defined as an acceptable range of GPA GPA
abbr.
grade point average

Noun 1. GPA - a measure of a student's academic achievement at a college or university; calculated by dividing the total number of grade points received by the total number attempted
 (2.0 or above). Relative achievement was measured by grade point average.

Several measures were administered both to students with and without LD. The achievement, process, and metacognitive measures were selected due to their strong theoretical basis and robust validity and reliability in published studies. All experimental measures had acceptable internal consistency In statistics and research, internal consistency is a measure based on the correlations between different items on the same test (or the same subscale on a larger test). It measures whether several items that propose to measure the same general construct produce similar scores.  reliability alpha ranges, .78-.98.

CLASSIFICATION MEASURES

Grade Point Average

Each student provided his or her current grade point average as it appeared in his or her transcripts at the time of the study. GPA ranges were 2.0-3.8, on a 4-point scale. GPA was selected as a measure of college success because it reflects adaptation to the college learning environment. We assumed that the additive nature of GPA measures success in specific courses as well as adaptation to the overall requirements associated with college, in addition to the ability to find a fit between individuals and their chosen academic path. GPA can be influenced extensively by metacognitive strategies, self-regulation, and motivational strategies (Garcia & Pintrich, 1993), mainly because it allows the execution and constant improvement of strategies.

Phonological Processing

Word attack. The Word Attack subtest of the Woodcock woodcock: see snipe.
woodcock

Any of five species (family Scolopacidae) of plump, sharp-billed migratory birds of damp, dense woodlands in North America, Europe, and Asia.
 Reading Mastery Test (Woodcock, 1987) assessed participants' phonological decoding, independent of semantics or comprehension, through a series of pseudo-words.

Phonological processing. Two subtests of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1998) were used: Sight Word Efficiency and Phonemic pho·ne·mic  
adj.
1. Of or relating to phonemes.

2. Of or relating to phonemics.

3. Serving to distinguish phonemes or distinctive features.
 Decoding Efficiency. The efficiency score measured the rate of as well as participants' ability to engage successfully with text under time constraints In law, time constraints are placed on certain actions and filings in the interest of speedy justice, and additionally to prevent the evasion of the ends of justice by waiting until a matter is moot. . Specifically, the Sight Word Efficiency subtest assessed the number of real printed words participants accurately identified within 45 seconds. The Phonemic Decoding Efficiency subtest measured the number of pronounceable pro·nounce  
v. pro·nounced, pro·nounc·ing, pro·nounc·es

v.tr.
1.
a. To use the organs of speech to make heard (a word or speech sound); utter.

b.
 printed pseudo-words that were accurately decoded within 45 seconds.

COMPARISON MEASURES

Working Memory

Phrase sequence. This subtest of the Swanson Cognitive Processing Test (S-CPT; Swanson, 1995) measured the verbal working memory capacity/efficiency of participants. Before recall, participants were asked a process question to interfere with short-term memory. It required participants to recall an increasing sequence of independent phrases.

Story retelling re·tell·ing  
n.
A new account or an adaptation of a story: a retelling of a Roman myth. 
. This subtest of the S-CPT (Swanson, 1995) measured working memory for connected text. The test requires participants to recall a series of episodes in a paragraph in their original order after answering a process question.

Sentence span measure. This test measures verbal working memory. It was originally used by Daneman and Carpenter (1980) and adapted by Swanson, Cochran, and Ewers (1989). Participants were asked to attend to sentences read by the examiner. After listening to the sentences, participants answered a process question based on the sentences and were then asked to recall the last word in each sentence in correct order. While the stop rule of the original test was retained, scoring was altered to add reliability and discrimination. Under the altered scoring rule In decision theory a score function, or scoring rule, is a measure of someone's performance when they are repeatedly making decisions under uncertainty. For example, a TV weather forecaster may give the probability of rain every day. , each sequence recalled correctly was awarded 1 point.

Speed of processing. Rapid automatic naming (RAN) is a measure of processing speed that has been hypothesized to be a deficit in reading disabilities (Wolf & Bowers Bowers is a surname, and may refer to
  • Betty Bowers
  • Bryan Bowers
  • Charles Bowers
  • Claude Bowers
  • Dane Bowers
  • David A. Bowers
  • Elizabeth Crocker Bowers
  • Graham Bowers
  • Henry Francis Bowers
  • Henry Robertson Bowers, (1883 - 1912), polar explorer
, 1999). In this study, RAN was measured by timed letter recognition and digit recognition subtests of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTTOP; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999).

Short-term memory. The Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised WAIS-R Psychology A measure of a person's cognitive abilities. See Psychological tests.  (WAIS-R WAIS-R Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, see there ; Wechsler, 1981) was used to assess short-term verbal memory. In the digits-forwards segment participants were asked to repeat a series of numbers presented verbally one per second. In the digit-backwards section of the subtest, participants were asked to repeat the digits in reverse order.

Olson Orthographic or·tho·graph·ic   also or·tho·graph·i·cal
adj.
1. Of or relating to orthography.

2. Spelled correctly.

3. Mathematics Having perpendicular lines.
 Test. This test measures the efficiency of letter and digit recognition by measuring visual recognition of patterns that occurred before phonological processing. In this time-limited test (30 s.), participants were asked to match a letter/number pattern from a column on the left to one of four options on the right.

Semantic Processing

Word classes. This subtest of the Clinical Evaluation clinical evaluation Medtalk An evaluation of whether a Pt has symptoms of a disease, is responding to treatment, or is having adverse reactions to therapy  Language Fundamentals-3 (CELF-3; The Psychological Corporation, 1994) assessed participants' ability to make semantic associations. A series of 3-4 words are presented orally once. Participants are then asked to repeat the words that go together best.

Semantic fluency. In this test (Harrison, Buxton, Husain, & Wise, 2000), participants were asked to recall as many animals as they could in 90 seconds. This experimental measure assessed participants' ability to make connections based on existing knowledge. While specialized knowledge might play a role in some cases (e.g., zoo keepers, park rangers A park ranger is a person charged with protecting and preserving protected parklands, forests (then called a forest ranger), wilderness areas, as well as other natural resources and protected cultural resources. , zoologists), using animals as a common category should present no knowledge limitations under the time constraints of the test. Participants were instructed not to repeat or use plural forms Noun 1. plural form - the form of a word that is used to denote more than one
plural

relation - (usually plural) mutual dealings or connections among persons or groups; "international relations"
 of an animal name. The dependent measure was the total number of correct names.

Vocabulary. Vocabulary was assessed using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R PPVT-R Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised ; Dunn & Dunn, 1981).

Spatial reasoning. The Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices Test (Raven, 1986) is a general reasoning test assumed to measure nonverbal intelligence Noun 1. nonverbal intelligence - intelligence that is manifested in the performance of tasks requiring little or no use of language
intelligence - the ability to comprehend; to understand and profit from experience
. Participants identified a partial figure for a series of other partial figures that complete a given matrix. The patterns increase in difficulty as the test progresses.

Reading

Word identification. This subtest of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised (Woodcock, 1980) required participants to read isolated words. Words presented to the participant became more complex phonologically and orthographically or·tho·graph·ic   also or·tho·graph·i·cal
adj.
1. Of or relating to orthography.

2. Spelled correctly.

3. Mathematics Having perpendicular lines.
.

Reading comprehension. The Passage Comprehension subtest of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test (Woodcock, 1987) measured participants' ability to understand a passage by requiring them to complete a cloze cloze  
adj.
Based on or being a test of reading comprehension in which the test taker is asked to supply words that have been systematically deleted from a text.



[Alteration of closure.]

Adj.
 exercise by coming up with a word that agreed with the syntactic structure, the context, and specific content of the given passage.

Metacognition

Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ MSLQ Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire
MSLQ Medical Science Liaison Quarterly
: Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993). The MSLQ is a self-report, 7-point rating scale designed to assess motivational strategies and use of learning strategies by college students. The motivation scales measure three broad areas: (a) value (intrinsic and extrinsic EVIDENCE, EXTRINSIC. External evidence, or that which is not contained in the body of an agreement, contract, and the like.
     2. It is a general rule that extrinsic evidence cannot be admitted to contradict, explain, vary or change the terms of a contract or of a
 goal orientation, task value); (b) expectancy (control beliefs about learning, self-efficacy); and (c) affect (test anxiety). The learning strategies section is composed of nine scales that can be distinguished as cognitive, metacognitive, and resource management strategies.

RESULTS

The means and standard deviations In statistics, the average amount a number varies from the average number in a series of numbers.

(statistics) standard deviation - (SD) A measure of the range of values in a set of numbers.
 for all measures are shown by category in Tables 3-5. The first analysis examined achievement differences between students with LD and those without LD. The second analysis focused on differences between the groups in cognitive and metacognitive measures, using a multivariate analysis multivariate analysis,
n a statistical approach used to evaluate multiple variables.

multivariate analysis,
n a set of techniques used when variation in several variables has to be studied simultaneously.
 of variance (MANOVA MANOVA Multivariate Analysis of the Variance ). The final analysis focused on compensatory mechanisms compensatory mechanisms Cardiac pacing Physiologic responsiveness of cardiovascular system whereby it changes its function and characteristics to ↑ or ↓ cardiac output. See Cardiac output.  by comparing the effects of strategy use for students with and without LD. Because age differences emerged between the groups, age was used as a covariate in subsequent analyses. Univariate analyses were not conducted to minimize type I error rates.

Achievement

The descriptive statistics in Table 3 show the results from achievement measures. A MANOVA on achievement measures failed to reveal a significant difference, F (3,36)=.67, p>.05.

Cognitive Measures

The cognitive measures were analyzed in each of the five clusters: word reading, working memory, semantic processing, processing speed, and general processing.

Word reading. A MANOVA was conducted with disability status (LD vs. NLD) as the fixed factors and all four word-reading tasks (Word Attack, Word Identification, Decoding Efficiency, and Sight Word Efficiency) as outcomes. The overall effect was significant, F(4,35)=31.28, p<.001, Wilks' [LAMBDA The Greek letter "L," which is used as a symbol for "wavelength." A lambda is a particular frequency of light, and the term is widely used in optical networking. Sending "multiple lambdas" down a fiber is the same as sending "multiple frequencies" or "multiple colors. ]=.22.

Working memory. Results of the MANCOVA MANCOVA Multivariate Analysis of Covariance  were not significant for ability group, F(3,35)=.73, p=.54, Wilks' [LAMBDA]=.94.

Semantic processing. The overall MANCOVA for disability status (LD vs. NLD) was significant for the two semantic processing tasks, F(1,37)=3.29, p=.05, Wilks' [LAMBDA]=.85.

Speed of processing. The MANCOVA was significant for the group effect, F(3,36)=9.67, p<.0001, Wilks' [LAMBDA]=.55. As illustrated in Table 4, students with LD were slower in all the tasks associated with processing speed.

General processing. The results of the Raven were examined using an ANCOVA ANCOVA Analysis of Covariance . The difference between students with and without LD was not significant, F(1,38)=1.6, p>.05.

Cognitive Performance

Composite scores were created from standardized scores (M=100; SD=15) in each cognitive domain: word level reading, working memory, semantic processing, and speed of processing. Reliability of the composites was .90 for word level reading, .75 for working memory, .60 for semantic processing, and .83 for speed of processing. The profiles in Figure 1 show significant difference in all domains except for the composite working memory score. This finding occurred because both groups were comparable in reading comprehension. That is, reported correlations between reading comprehension and working memory were high (r's in .80 range; see Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). Overall, however, the cognitive functioning cognitive function Neurology Any mental process that involves symbolic operations–eg, perception, memory, creation of imagery, and thinking; CFs encompasses awareness and capacity for judgment  on most domains associated with reading was lower for students with LD.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Metacognitive Measures

Motivation. A MANCOVA was conducted to estimate a possible difference in motivation between students with LD and NLD in four motivational subtests of the MSLQ: internal goals, external goals, task value, and control. Results showed a nonsignificant non·sig·nif·i·cant  
adj.
1. Not significant.

2. Having, producing, or being a value obtained from a statistical test that lies within the limits for being of random occurrence.
 main effect for disability status, F(3,36)=.94, p=.43, Wilks' [LAMBDA] =.93.

Expectancy components. High self-efficacy and high perceived control were positive indicators of metacognition. That is, a high score on these measures corresponded to a high sense of self-efficacy and control. The third factor in this analysis was test anxiety. Here a high score indicated high anxiety, which corresponded to lower self-efficacy and a low sense of control. For this analysis, test anxiety scores were inverted inverted

reverse in position, direction or order.


inverted L block
a pattern of local filtration anesthesia commonly used in laparotomy in the ox.
 to indicate low anxiety with a high score and high anxiety with a low score.

Because the overall analysis showed that age was not a significant covariate, a MANOVA was computed on three expectancy values. A significant group effect emerged, F(3,36)=4.97, p=.005, Wilks [LAMBDA]=.71. Results indicated an overall lower expectancy value for students with LD compared with NLD students. The results of the univariate analyses (see Table 5) showed that perceived control and test anxiety were lower for students with LD. Self-efficacy for both students with LD and NLD were statistically comparable.

Learning strategies. A MANCOVA on the MSLQ learning strategies (rehearsal, critical thinking, organization, and elaboration) was computed. Disability status was nonsignificant, F(4,34)=1.10, p=.39, Wilks' [LAMBDA]=.89.

Self-regulation of learning. A MANCOVA was conducted on the five self-regulation subtests of the MSLQ (manage resources, manage time and resources, effort regulation, peer learning, and help seeking). A significant group effect was found, F(5,33)=2.67, p=.04, Wilks' [LAMBDA]=.71. The overall test indicated a significant higher use of self-regulation by students with LD than NLD students.

In summary, results of the above analyses showed that students with LD were not statistically different from NLD students in terms of motivation, metacognitive strategies, and reading strategies. However, they were higher in number of hours of study and overall self-regulation compared to NLD students. In addition, students with LD had significantly lower expectancy than NLD students.

The Relationship Between Metacognition and Achievement

Although differences in learning strategies between groups were not detected, an important question was whether strategy use was related to achievement for the LD group alone. Because the correlation between strategy use and GPA for the LD group was significant, r=.57 p=.006, further analysis was necessary to examine strategy use.

Compensation through metacognitive strategies. All students were divided into groups of low and high strategy use based on the MSLQ combined score for strategy. In the NLD group, 11 students had low strategy use and 9 had high strategy use. For the students with LD, 11 fell in the high-strategy use group and 9 in the low-strategy use group. The age covariate was not significant; therefore an ANOVA anova

see analysis of variance.

ANOVA Analysis of variance, see there
 was computed.

The results indicated that strategy use yielded a significant main effect, F(1,36)=6.53, p<.05, MSE MSE Mouse (computer)
MSE Materials Science & Engineering
MSE Mean Squared Error
MSE Mean Square Error
MSE Master of Science in Engineering
MSE Manufacturing Systems Engineering
MSE Mechanically Stabilized Earth
=.19. This finding indicated that high strategy use was associated with higher GPA. More important was the interaction Disability Status x Strategy Use, F(1,36)=5.63, p<.05, MSE=.19. Students with LD with high strategy use had higher achievement than that of NLD students.

Help seeking. A number of studies have pointed to help seeking as a key strategy in regulating learning (e.g., Branker, 1997; Merladet, 2001; Ruban, 2000). In this analysis the age covariate was not significant. An ANOVA indicated a main effect for help seeking, F(1,36)=7.50, p<.01, MSE=.18, and an interaction between disability status and help seeking, F(1,36)= 5.61, p<.05, MSE=.18. The interaction indicated that that help seeking was not associated with increased achievement for NLD students but was highly associated with achievement for students with LD.

DISCUSSION

This study assessed whether college students with LD who have a phonological deficit The phonological deficit hypothesis is a prevalent neurological explanation for the cause of reading difficulties and dyslexia. It stems from evidence that individuals with dyslexia tend to do poorly on tests with measure their ability to decode nonsense words using conventional  compensate for their processing difficulties by relying on increasing levels of metacognition. Grade point average and achievement were found to be related to increased metacognitive learning strategies and help-seeking activities in students with LD.

The results point to four additional areas of importance. First, students with and without LD performed equally well across the three achievement measures used: GPA, reading comprehension, and vocabulary. The nonsignificant difference in achievement between the students with LD and their NED peers corroborated cor·rob·o·rate  
tr.v. cor·rob·o·rat·ed, cor·rob·o·rat·ing, cor·rob·o·rates
To strengthen or support with other evidence; make more certain. See Synonyms at confirm.
 Vogel and Adelman's (1992) and Wilczeki's (1993) findings, showing that achievement levels for college students with LD in their junior and senior years were equal to those of their NLD peers.

Second, college students with LD in this study were still suffering from significant difficulties in basic processes connected with reading. Thus, deficits were present in all aspects of their word reading, pseudo-word reading, real-word reading, and rate of reading, even though reading comprehension was in the average range. In addition, speed of processing results were similar to the results of the phonological processing and word-level reading in general. These ongoing difficulties support Bruck's (1990, 1992, 1993) findings of continuing phonological deficits in individuals with a childhood diagnosis of dyslexia. Students with disabilities performed lower in the two hypothesized deficits connected with LD, word reading (d=3.8) and speed of processing (d=1.77). However, phonological and speed of processing were not the only cognitive domains in which students with disabilities performed significantly lower than their peers without LD. They also performed lower in semantic processing and short-term memory.

These significant differences across the four domains support a view of a more generalized deficit than only phonology in students with LD. Thus, the results highlight the difficulty in naming one or two specific deficit processes when defining LD when both groups are comparable in reading comprehension.

Third, the motivation of students with LD was comparable to that of their NED peers. Their self-assessment extrinsic and intrinsic motivations were the same. However, these findings were qualified when the analysis focused on anxiety, help seeking, and self-regulation.

Finally, the students with LD had a significantly lower expectancy value and high anxiety, similar to the findings reported by Stevens (2001). A possible explanation is protective pessimism (Cantor & Norem, 1989; Norem & Cantor 1986). According to according to
prep.
1. As stated or indicated by; on the authority of: according to historians.

2. In keeping with: according to instructions.

3.
 Cantor, some individuals motivate themselves to work harder and protect themselves from possible failure by being a priori a priori

In epistemology, knowledge that is independent of all particular experiences, as opposed to a posteriori (or empirical) knowledge, which derives from experience.
 and unrealistically pessimistic about their prospects. The pessimistic view of future achievement promotes self-regulation of task attainment; it promotes planning, thought, and effort. While defensive pessimism is beneficial in the short run, possible negative outcomes relate to self-image, long-term satisfaction, and stress levels (Cantor, 1989).

Test anxiety had a very different impact for students with and without LD. Specifically, students with LD have a long history of difficulty and failure. Thus, adverse physical and psychological reaction to a testing situation is more a result of past experiences than a disproportional dis·pro·por·tion·al  
adj.
Disproportionate.



dispro·por
 reaction. Results indicate that for students with LD, normal test anxiety is not associated with actual performance, but serve as protective pessimism.

Students with LD reported seeking help more often than their NLD peers. They made conscious attempts to recruit family, friends, instructors, and formal agencies to support their learning. In self-regulation of learning and time management, students with LD had more consistent and varied use. Students with LD spent considerably longer hours on schoolwork than their NLD peers, most over 30 hours a week. In surveys students with LD reported starting to prepare early, avoiding last-minute learning (cramming The unauthorized addition of services to your telephone bill such as an 800 number that you never ordered. The charges are usually noted on the bill, but are identified in a cryptic manner and/or are printed in a place that is easy to overlook. See slamming. ), and, in some cases, adopting a pre-exam ritual.

Do successful students in college rely on metacognition to compensate for weak reading skills? A direct answer to this question cannot be given. This is because students with LD indicated using the same level of problem solving problem solving

Process involved in finding a solution to a problem. Many animals routinely solve problems of locomotion, food finding, and shelter through trial and error.
 as their NLD peers during academic reading, after accounting for age differences. Students with LD were not more strategic than their NLD peers about preparation for tests, in lessons, and in writing papers. Further, the use of strategies in learning and reading to predict grade point average was not significantly different between student with LD and NLD.

However, differences in metacognitive performance levels in self-regulation of learning and expectancy levels indicate a possible compensatory route for students with LD. Results supported the hypothesis that students with LD benefit more from high strategy use than NLD students. Students with LD who were strategic about their learning had as high a grade point average as NLD students who were strategic and significantly higher achievement than students who were low strategy users regardless of disability status. Students with LD and low strategy use, on the other hand, had a grade point average lower than all other groups, showing that strategy use is a key to the success of students with LD.

The study also showed a similar pattern in help seeking. That is, students with LD and low help-seeking ability had much lower grade point averages than all other groups. At the same time the help-seeking level for students without LD did not relate to GPA level.

Because compensation was observable in the achievement level of students with LD, the question about the possible paths to academic success despite deficiencies remains unanswered. In their theoretical model of life span compensation, Baltes and Baltes (1990) suggested two nonmutually exclusive paths: cognitive and metacognitive. Cognitive compensation stipulates that adults with lower processing ability (older individuals in Baltes & Baltes, 1990; students with LD in the present case) use more efficient processes or general processing ability (e.g., working memory) to compensate for their difficulties whenever possible. The model offered by Baltes and Baltes (1990) is a general model of compensation. To operationalize cognitive compensation related to reading, there is a need for a model describing the processes related to reading and the interactions involved with them. More specific models of reading suggested by Perfetti (1988) and Conners and Olson (1990) suggest that in order to compensate for difficulties in word reading, individuals with LD have to rely on vocabulary knowledge and working memory. The analyses of variance in the present study showed a pattern of results compatible with the models suggested by Perfetti and Conners and Olson.

Implications

The results have implications for both the transition of students with LD into postsecondary education and the support they receive there. Transition should emphasize the realities of college life, as one student with LD in this study put it, "It's not like high school. Most classes are so big nobody cares about you; they do not even know who you are. Nobody will say anything until they are ready to kick you out." Entering higher education higher education

Study beyond the level of secondary education. Institutions of higher education include not only colleges and universities but also professional schools in such fields as law, theology, medicine, business, music, and art.
 shifts the burden of social ecologies While the field of ecology focuses on the relationships between organisms and their environments, social ecology is a philosophy concerned with the relationships between humans and their environments.  from established networks of family and school to the individual. It may be beneficial for some students to start at the junior college level with added support and then get into the more demanding and less supportive four-year colleges. Help-seeking and social skills are integral to establishing the support systems crucial to academic success. Time management skills and decision making about resources are also important and should be part of the transition into higher education as well as an ongoing evaluation of progress. Constantly redefining the learning strategies in reading, test taking, and writing that work for the individual is important in establishing long-term success. Finally, it is important to emphasize, for both students and educators, that strategies are general principles adjusted to fit specific demands and content, not a set of always-applicable skills.

Requests for reprints should be addressed to: Guy Trainin, 118 Henzlik Hall, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68588; gtrainin@unl.edu.

REFERENCES

Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, Pub. L. No. 101-336, 2, 104 Stat.328 (1991).

Adelman, P. B., & Vogel, S. A. (1993). Issues in program evaluation Program evaluation is a formalized approach to studying and assessing projects, policies and program and determining if they 'work'. Program evaluation is used in government and the private sector and it's taught in numerous universities. . In S. A. Vogel & P. B. Adelman (Eds.), Success for college students with learning disabilities (pp. 323-343). New York New York, state, United States
New York, Middle Atlantic state of the United States. It is bordered by Vermont, Massachusetts, Connecticut, and the Atlantic Ocean (E), New Jersey and Pennsylvania (S), Lakes Erie and Ontario and the Canadian province of
: Springer-Verlag.

Apthorp, H. (1995). Phonetic pho·net·ic
adj.
1. Of or relating to phonetics.

2. Representing the sounds of speech with a set of distinct symbols, each designating a single sound.
 coding and reading in college students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 342-352.

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences behavioral sciences,
n.pl those sciences devoted to the study of human and animal behavior.
 (pp. 1-34). New York: Cambridge University Press Cambridge University Press (known colloquially as CUP) is a publisher given a Royal Charter by Henry VIII in 1534, and one of the two privileged presses (the other being Oxford University Press). .

Branker, C. R. (1997). An inquiry into college students with learning disabilities: Why are they successful? (Doctoral Dissertation, North Carolina State University History

Main article: History of North Carolina State University
The North Carolina General Assembly founded NC State on March 7, 1887 as a land-grant college under the name North Carolina College of Agriculture and Mechanic Arts.
, 2001). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 2097.

Bruck, M. (1985). The adult functioning of children with specific learning disabilities: A follow-up study. In I. E. Siegel (Ed.), Advances in applied developmental psychology developmental psychology

Branch of psychology concerned with changes in cognitive, motivational, psychophysiological, and social functioning that occur throughout the human life span.
 (pp. 91-129). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Bruck, M. (1990). Word recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439-454.

Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness Phonological awareness is the conscious sensitivity to the sound structure of language. It includes the ability to auditorily distinguish parts of speech, such as syllables and phonemes.  deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874-886.

Bruck, M. (1993). Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 171-184.

Conners, F. A., & Olson, R. K. (1990). Reading comprehension in dyslexic dys·lex·ic or dys·lec·tic
adj.
Of or relating to dyslexia.

n.
A person affected by dyslexia.
 and normal readers: A component-skills analysis. In D. A. Balota, F. d'Arcais, & B. Giovanni (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 557-579). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.

Dalke, C. (1988). Woodcock-Johnson psycho-educational test battery profiles: A comparative study of college freshmen with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 567-570.

Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450-466.

Dixon, R. A., & Baeckman, L. (1995). Concepts of compensation: Integrated, differentiated, and Janus-faced. In R. A. Dixon & L. Baeckman (Eds.), Compensating for psychological deficits and declines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 3-19). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1994). Regulating motivation and cognition cognition

Act or process of knowing. Cognition includes every mental process that may be described as an experience of knowing (including perceiving, recognizing, conceiving, and reasoning), as distinguished from an experience of feeling or of willing.
 in the classroom: The role of self-schemas and self-regulatory strategies. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational applications (pp. 101-124). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Harrison, J. E., Buxton, P., Husain, M., & Wise, R. (2000). Short test of semantic and phonological fluency: Normal performance, validity and test-retest reliability test-retest reliability Psychology A measure of the ability of a psychologic testing instrument to yield the same result for a single Pt at 2 different test periods, which are closely spaced so that any variation detected reflects reliability of the instrument . British Journal of Clinical Psychology The Journal of Clinical Psychology, founded in 1945, is a peer-reviewed forum devoted to psychological research, assessment, and practice. Published eight times a year, the Journal , 39, 181-191.

Henderson, C. (1999). College freshmen with disabilities. Washington, DC: American Council on Education Established in 1918, the American Council on Education (ACE) is a United States organization comprising over 1,800 accredited, degree-granting colleges and universities and higher education-related associations, organizations, and corporations. .

Kenper, S. (1986). Imitation of complex syntactic constructions by elderly adults. Applied Psycholinguistics psycholinguistics, the study of psychological states and mental activity associated with the use of language. An important focus of psycholinguistics is the largely unconscious application of grammatical rules that enable people to produce and comprehend intelligible , 7, 277-287.

Lefly, D., & Pennington, B. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143-162.

McGuire, J. M., Hall, D., & Litt, V. A. (1991). A field based study of the direct service needs of college students with learning disabilities. Journal of College Student Development Journal of College Student Development is an academic journal founded in 1959 and is the official publication of the American College Personnel Association. The journal publishes scholarly articles and reviews from a wide variety of academic fields related to college , 32, 101-108.

Merladet, J. (2001). Demographic, cognitive and academic factors that predict graduation for learning disabled college students. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota (body, education) University of Minnesota - The home of Gopher.

http://umn.edu/.

Address: Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA.
, 2001). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 2596.

Newman, L., & Cameto, R. (1993). What makes a difference? Factors related to postsecondary school attendance for young people with disabilities. Menlo Park Menlo Park.

1 Residential city (1990 pop. 28,040), San Mateo co., W Calif.; inc. 1874. Electronic equipment and aerospace products are manufactured in the city. Menlo College and a Stanford Univ. research institute are there.

2 Uninc.
, CA: SRI International (company) SRI International - One of the world's largest contract research firms. Founded in 1946 in conjuction with Stanford University as the Stanford Research Institute, they later became fully independent and were incorporated as a non-profit organisation under U.S. .

Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology The Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (often referred to as JPSP) is a monthly psychology journal of the American Psychological Association. It is considered one of the top journals in the fields of social and personality psychology. , 51, 1208-1217.

Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1989). Defensive pessimism and stress and coping. Social Cognition Social cognition is the study of how people process social information, especially its encoding, storage, retrieval, and application to social situations. Social cognition’s focus on information processing has many affinities with its sister discipline, cognitive psychology. , 7, 92-112.

Pennington, B., McCabe, L., Smith, S., Lefly, D., Bookman, M., Kimberling, W., & Lubs, H. (1986). Spelling errors in adults with a familial dyslexia. Child Development, 57, 1001-1013.

Perfetti, C. A. (1988). Verbal efficiency in reading ability. In M. Daneman & G. E. Mackinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (vol. 6, pp. 109-143). San Diego San Diego (săn dēā`gō), city (1990 pop. 1,110,549), seat of San Diego co., S Calif., on San Diego Bay; inc. 1850. San Diego includes the unincorporated communities of La Jolla and Spring Valley. Coronado is across the bay. , CA: Academic Press.

Pintrich, P. R. (1994) Student motivation in the college classroom. In K. W. Prichard & R McLaran Sawyer (Eds.), Handbook off college teaching: Theory and applications (pp. 23-43). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity In psychometrics, predictive validity is the extent to which a scale predicts scores on some criterion measure.

For example, the validity of a cognitive test for job performance is the correlation between test scores and, for example, supervisor performance ratings.
 of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational & Psychological Measurement, 53, 801-813.

The Psychological Corporation. (1994). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (3rd ed.). San Antonio San Antonio (săn ăntō`nēō, əntōn`), city (1990 pop. 935,933), seat of Bexar co., S central Tex., at the source of the San Antonio River; inc. 1837. , TX: Harcourt Brace.

Ransby, M. J., & Swanson, H. L. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnosis of Dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 538-555.

Raven, J. C. (1986). Raven Colored Progressive Matrices. London: Lewis.

Reis, S. M., McGuire, J. N., & Neu, T. W. (2000). Compensation strategies used by high ability students with learning disabilities who succeed in college. Gifted Child gifted child

Child naturally endowed with a high degree of general mental ability or extraordinary ability in a specific domain. Although the designation of giftedness is largely a matter of administrative convenience, the best indications of giftedness are often those
 Quarterly, 44, 123-134.

Ritter, S. S. (2000). Cognitive, achievement, and background variables predicting diagnosis of the college student with a reading based learning disability. (Doctoral Dissertation, The Ohio State University Ohio State University, main campus at Columbus; land-grant and state supported; coeducational; chartered 1870, opened 1873 as Ohio Agricultural and Mechanical College, renamed 1878. There are also campuses at Lima, Mansfield, Marion, and Newark. , 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 498.

Ruban, L. M. (2000). Patterns of self-regulated learning The term self-regulated can be used to describe learning that is guided by metacognition, strategic action (planning, monitoring, and evaluating personal progress against a standard), and motivation to learn  and academic achievement among university students with and without learning disabilities. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut The University of Connecticut is the State of Connecticut's land-grant university. It was founded in 1881 and serves more than 27,000 students on its six campuses, including more than 9,000 graduate students in multiple programs.

UConn's main campus is in Storrs, Connecticut.
, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 1296.

Salthouse, T. A. (1992). Mechanisms of age-cognition relations in adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Scarborough, H. (1984). Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 329-348.

Smitely, B. L. (2001). Factors predicting academic adjustment among college students with learning disabilities. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Miami This article is about the university in Coral Gables, Florida. For the university in Oxford, Ohio, see Miami University.

The University of Miami (also known as Miami of Florida,[2] UM,[3] or just The U
.

Stevens, S. A. (2001). Test anxiety and beliefs about testing in college students with and without learning disabilities. (Doctoral Dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst US News and World Report's 2008 edition of America's Best Colleges ranked UMass Amherst as one of the top 100 universities in the nation, placing it at #96, and ranking it the joint 46th amongst Public Universities. , 2001). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 2676.

Swanson, H. L. (1996). The Swanson Cognitive Process Test (S-CPT). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Swanson, H. L., Cochran, K. F., & Ewers, C. A. (1989). Working memory in skilled and less skilled readers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 145-156.

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1998). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Evanston, IL: Cognitive Concepts.

Vogel, S. A., & Adelman, P. B. (1992). The success of college students with learning disabilities: Factors related to educational attainment Educational attainment is a term commonly used by statisticans to refer to the highest degree of education an individual has completed.[1]

The US Census Bureau Glossary defines educational attainment as "the highest level of education completed in terms of the
. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 430-441.

Wagner, M. M., Blackorby, J., Cameto, R., & Newman, L. (1993). What makes a difference? Influences on postschool outcomes of youth with disabilities. The third comprehensive report from the National Longitudinal Transition Study of Special Education Students. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.

Wagner, M. M., & Blackorby, J. (1996). Transition from high school to work or college: How special education students fare. Special Education for Students with Disabilities, 6, 103-120.

Wagner, P. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP CTOPP Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing ). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wilczenski, F. L. (1993). Comparison of academic performances, graduation rates, and timing of dropout (1) On magnetic media, a bit that has lost its strength due to a surface defect or recording malfunction. If the bit is in an audio or video file, it might be detected by the error correction circuitry and either corrected or not, but if not, it is often not noticed by the human  for LD and nonLD college students. College Student Journal, 27, 184-194.

Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438.

Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Zimmerman, B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key subprocesses? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 307-313.

Guy Trainin, Ph.D., University of Nebraska-Lincoln.

H. Lee Swanson, Ph.D., University of California-Riverside.
Table 1
Class Standing and Major Field of Study for LD and NLD Samples

Class Standing     NLD    LD      Total

Freshman            1      4        5
Sophomore           2      1        3
Junior              5      3        8
Senior             11     10       21
MA                  1      2        3

Major
Humanities          7      7       14
Social Science      7      8       15
Science             6      5       11

Note. Claus standing, [chi square](4)=3.01,
p=.56. Major, [chi square](2)=.16, p=.92.

Table 2
Means and SD of Learning Disability Indicators by Group

                                NLD            LD        Comparison

Word Attack (WRMT)          106.7 (9.1)    84.8 (6.8)    NLD>LD ***
Digit Naming Speed (CTOP)   104.5 (13.1)   89.2 (16.4)   NLD>LD **
Nonverbal
  Intelligence (Raven PM)   110.2 (10.1)   98.8 (7.2)    NLD>LD **

Note. Standardized score: M=100; SD=15; ** p<.01; *** p<.005.

Table 3
Observed Means, Standard Errors, Comparisons,
and Effect Sizes of Achievement Measures

                         NLD (n=20)        LD (n=20)

                         M       SD      M       SD

GPA                      2.88    .37     2.83     .58
Reading
  Comprehension-WRMT   104.7    7.7    104.7    12.1
Vocabulary-PPVT        102.8    8.3     98.6    12.1

                       Comparison       ES
                                         D

GPA                    NLD=LD          0.16
Reading
  Comprehension-WRMT   NLD=LD          0.01
Vocabulary-PPVT        NLD=LD          0.40

Note. GPA is on 4-point scale. Reading comprehension
and PPVT scores are standard scores: M=100; SD=15.

* p<.05.

Table 4
Means, Standard Deviations, Least-Square
Means, and Effect Sizes of Cognitive Tasks

                                M (SD)                LSM

                            NLD          LD        NLD      LD

Sentence Span            5.3 (1.8)    4.1 (1.5)   102.3    97.6
Phrase Sequence          2.2 (1.1)    1.5 (1.2)   104.4    95.6
Story Retell             5.9 (2.2)    6.3 (2.3)    98.6   101.3
Sentence Span            5.3 (1.8)    4.1 (1.5)   102.3    97.6
Phrase Sequence          2.2 (1.1)    1.5 (1.2)   104.4    95.6
Story Retell             5.9 (2.2)    6.3 (2.3)    98.6   101.3
Semantic Recall         29.4 (6.0)   26.8 (7.5)   102.7    97.3
CELF Word Classes       17.4 (1.4)   15.5 (3.0)   105.7    94.3
RAN Letters (a)         24.4 (3.8)   29.5 (6.9)   104.5    89.3
RAN Digits (a)          22.3 (4.8)   28.8 (8.5)    99.8    88.4
Digit Span              18.6 (4.5)   14.7 (2.7)   107.0    93.0
Word Attack             38.9 (2.5)   29.6 (4.2)   106.7    84.9
Word ID                 99.3 (2.4)   91.7 (8.9)   102.9    89.1
Decoding Efficiency     56.5 (5.4)   35.6(12.2)   111.1    88.9
Sight Word Efficiency   98.3 (3.9)   79.4(11.1)   111.1    88.9
Olson                   12.7 (2.4)    8.9 (3.3)   105.9    94.1
Raven                   17.7 (3.9)   13.2 (6.8)   107.6   101.4

Note. Least-square means are in standardized form, M=100;
SD=15, adjusted for covariates when applicable.

(a) This metric is time in seconds, inverting
the relationship between LD and NLD students.

Table 5
Means, Standard Deviations, and Least-Square Means of MSLQ Subscale

                                         M (SD)

                                    NLD          LD

Intrinsic Goal Orientation (a)   19.1 (2.5)   20.7 (5.4)
Extrinsic Goal Orientation       22.0 (3.6)   20.4 (4.3)
Task Valuea                      32.2 (3.8)   32.3 (5.4)
Perceived Control                19.7 (4.6)   19.3 (.81)
Self-Efficacy                    29.6 (4.8)   30.6 (l.2)
Test Anxiety                     17.4 (4.5)   15.8 (1.0)
Rehearsala                       19.7 (4.6)   19.3 (.81)
Elaborationa                     29.6 (4.8)   30.6 (1.2)
Organizationa                    17.4 (4.5)   15.8 (1.0)
Critical Thinking (a)            18.8 (5.2)   21.1 (1.5)
Manage Resource (a)              49.3 (7.5)   54.0 (1.96)
Manage Time/ Study (a)           35.7 (9.1)   41.0 (1.91)
Effort Regulation (a)            18.6 (4.1)   19.0 (1.24)
Peer Learning                    11.2 (3.5)   12.0 (.57)
Help-Seeking                     17.2 (4.3)   19.0 (.64)

                                             LSM

                                      NLD          LD

Intrinsic Goal Orientation (a)       100.6         99.4
Extrinsic Goal Orientation           102.9         97.0
Task Valuea                          103.6         96.4
Perceived Control                    104.6         95.4
Self-Efficacy                        101.2         98.8
Test Anxiety                         105.7         94.3
Rehearsala                           103.6         96.4
Elaborationa                         102.3         97.7
Organizationa                        105.1         94.8
Critical Thinking (a)                 99.9        100.1
Manage Resource (a)                   99.1        100.9
Manage Time/ Study (a)                98.4        101.6
Effort Regulation (a)                 97.1        102.8
Peer Learning                         97.9        102.1
Help-Seeking                          96.4        103.5

Note. Least-square means are in standardized form,
M=100; SD=15, adjusted for covariates when applicable.

(a) This metric is time in seconds, inverting
the relationship between LD and NLD students.
COPYRIGHT 2005 Council for Learning Disabilities
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2005, Gale Group. All rights reserved. Gale Group is a Thomson Corporation Company.

 Reader Opinion

Title:

Comment:



 

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Author:Swanson, H. Lee
Publication:Learning Disability Quarterly
Geographic Code:1USA
Date:Sep 22, 2005
Words:7081
Previous Article:Responsiveness to intervention and learning disabilities.
Next Article:Effect of acquisition rates on off-task behavior with children identified as having learning disabilities.
Topics:



Related Articles
Helping gifted students develop metacognitive awareness.
Emergent Metacognition: A Study of Preschoolers' Literate Behavior.
Learning Strategies and Hypermedia Instruction.
Motivational and attitudinal factors in college students with and without learning disabilities.
Off-line metacognition--a domain-specific retardation in young children with learning disabilities?
EPA2000: assessing off-line metacognition in mathematical problem solving.
Using metacognitive skills to improve 3rd graders' math problem solving.
Intentional Conceptual Change.
Understanding learning disabilities.
Motivational issues in learning disabilities: editors' introduction to special issue.

Terms of use | Copyright © 2014 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters